Academic
Achievement
www.TheCitizensWhoCare.org
|
Real Women
Don't Alienate Their Children from Their Fathers.
7:51
ManWomanMyth - Education -
Introduction
Gender
Gap
Leaving
Boys Behind: Gender Disparities in High Academic
Achievement1
(61 page pdf)
Gender
gaps among students revealed by
Ucas
Girls
Make Higher Grades than Boys in All School Subjects,
Analysis Finds
Why
Girls Tend to Get Better Grades Than Boys
Do
Why
Do More Women than Men Go to
College?
How
do we help boys close the academic gender
gap?
The
Reverse Gender Gap
Girls
lead boys in academic achievement
globally
Gender
proves large factor in academic
performance
Sex
differences in academic achievement are not related to
political, economic or social
equality
Gender
Segregation: Separate But
Effective?
Parents
aiming too high can harm child's academic
performance
The
Link Between Sports and Academic
Performance
Social
Networks Influence Academic Performance:
Study
The
Cost of Gender Inequality
Four-Day
School Week Can Improve Academic Performance, Policy Study
Finds
A
Degree Goal: To Close a Gender Gap That Favors
Women
Does
Gender Diversiity Improeve
Performance
The
Growing Gender Chasm In Education And The Misandric Myths
Associated With It
Gender
and Academic Achievement

Gender
differences in GCSE
(44 page pdf)
The
ABC of Gender Equality in Education: Aptitude, Behaviour,
Confidence
(182 page pdf)
Relationship
of second-year college student wellness behaviors to
academic achievement by gender, race/ethnicity, and
socioeconomic status.

Gender
Differences in Aspirations and
Attainment 
Parental
Divorce and Student Academic
Achievement
Four
Keys to Driving a Successful Change Management
Strategy
(104 page pdf)
Gender
differences in school achievement: The role of
self-regulation
Resources
Gender
Gap
Debates about gender and schooling have taken a
surprising turn in the past decade. After years of concern
that girls were being shortchanged in male-dominated
schools, especially in math and science, there has grown a
rising chorus of voices worrying about whether boys are the
ones in peril. With young women making up close to 60
percent of college students, critics like Richard Whitmire,
former USA Today editorial writer and author of Why Boys
Fail, worry that todays schoolswith their
emphasis on order, sitting still, and passive
learningare much better suited to girls than to boys.
Other authorities, such as Susan McGee Bailey, executive
director of the Wellesley Centers for Women at Wellesley
College and principal author of the 1992 AAUW report How
Schools Shortchange Girls, reject such concerns and instead
contend that ingrained sexism and gender roles continue to
hamper K12 schooling for both boys and girls. What
does the evidence say? And what does all of this mean for
policy proposals like single-sex schooling or teacher
hiring? In this forum, Whitmire and Bailey sort through
these questions.
Education Next: Whats the
evidence that boys are doing less well in school than
girls?
Richard Whitmire: Dropout and
graduation rates, grades, and many test scores show boys
faring poorly compared to girls (see Figure 1). But I prefer
a simpler measure. Students need at least one year of
posthigh school study to survive in todays
marketplace, the goal wisely set this year by President
Obama. In truth, they should complete two years of college.
When that level of achievement is broken out by gender, men
are faring badly. They go to college at lower rates and then
graduate at lower rates. Lets take Minnesota as an
example. The (St. Paul) Pioneer Press just published an
article on the gender gaps in that state. As of fall 2007,
degrees earned by gender were bachelors: 58 percent
female; masters: 69 percent female; PhD: 53 percent
female. Nationally, 58 percent of those earning
bachelors degrees and 62 percent of those earning
associates degrees are female.
For the most part this is happening
because K12 schools are shortchanging boys. Far too
many boys drop out before earning a high school diploma.
Worse, too many boys who do make it through high school are
either unprepared for or unmotivated to do college-level
work.
The conventional wisdom that women
need a college degree more than men was true at one time,
but is no longer. Economists at both the College Board and
the U.S. Department of Education agree: men and women may
earn different average salaries, but they get almost exactly
the same percentage bump-up in earnings for each degree
earned.
Those manufacturing jobs that men
could secure with only a high school degree have been
slipping away for years. In the current recession, that
trend picked up speed, with more than 80 percent of the
layoffs involving men. Now more than ever, men and women
have equal needs to earn degrees past high school, but far
more women than men are getting that message.
Susan McGee Bailey: Clearly,
all our students need strong preparation for the demands of
a high-tech, global world, but international data such as
those provided by TIMSS (Trends in International Mathematics
and Science Study) and PISA (Program for International
Student Assessment) show U.S. students of both sexes
performing in a mediocre fashion in comparison to their
counterparts in other industrialized nations.
Focusing on the lower college
completion rates for boys and blaming K12 educators is
too easy. First, the much smaller college-enrollment gap
compared to the larger degree gap raises questions about
college. College enrollments have been increasing for both
young women and young men since the 1970s, but the increase
for young women has been larger (see Figure 2a). In 1972, 53
percent of males and 46 percent of females enrolled in two-
or four-year colleges immediately after graduating from high
school; in 2007 the comparable figures were 66 percent of
males and 68 percent of females. Women now outpace men in
BA, MA, and PhD completion, but are significantly behind men
in MBAs and earn law and medical degrees at slightly lower
rates than men. Studies suggesting that men and women get
the same benefit from a degree obscure the critical reality
that women still earn less than men at every level (see
Figure 2b).
During the past 20 years, discussions
of educational equity have often fallen into an either/or
paradigm in which one group of students has been singled out
as the only group needing attention. Dropout rates
illustrate the dangers of focusing too narrowly. Dropout
rates have been declining for both girls and boys, with the
rate of decrease greater for girls as a group. But simply
looking at gender differences is not enough. Rates vary
considerably by race, ethnicity, and social class, and large
numbers of girls as well as boys leave school before earning
a high school diploma (see Figure 3). Educators are rightly
focused on ensuring high-quality instruction, developing new
and improved curricular materials, and creating more
engaging school environments. But educators alone cannot
address the multiple factors that influence students who
drop out, nor can they conduct the kinds of community
outreach that can help young people find alternative routes
to completing their education.
EN: Is it all boys who are
struggling or particular subsets of boys (like poor minority
boys)?
RW: Thats the challenge
raised by those who question whether boys are in trouble:
this is all about income and race, not gender, they argue.
Its true that the gender gaps are especially sharp in
urban areas. In July 2009, the Center for Labor Market
Studies at Northeastern University released a study that
tracked the students who graduated from Boston Public
Schools in 2007. The conclusion: for every 167 women in
four-year colleges there were only 100 men. Is poverty the
cause? The male and female students came from similar
streets and neighborhoods. Is race the only issue?
Thats not what the study uncovered. In fact, black
females were five percentage points more likely to pursue
further study after high school, including community
colleges, four-year colleges, and technical or vocational
schools, than white males.
Gender gaps are especially profound
for poor and minority males. Its what Chicago
researcher Melissa Roderick calls the genderization of
race. Roughly translated: you wont solve racial
learning gaps unless you tackle the gender gaps.
Unfortunately, school accountability regimes such as No
Child Left Behind keep educators fixated solely on learning
gaps associated with race and income.
Now lets shift to the
comfortable suburban districts, where both boys and girls go
on to college at a high rate. Educators there see few
problems, so they rarely break out the numbers by gender.
There are a few exceptions. When school officials in two
districts serving wealthy familiesEdina outside
Minneapolis and Wilmette outside Chicagotook a hard
look at their gender numbers, they found wide and growing
gaps. The Wilmette data were very specific, showing girls
ahead in both grades and test scores.
If nearly all the students there go to
college anyway, does this matter? I argue that it does. A
considerable number of those boys get into selective private
colleges due to gender preferences granted males by
admissions officers, a practice that is both concealed and
widespread. Uncovering the preferences is relatively easy.
Take the U.S. News & World Report data and sort
admission rates by gender. Still skeptical? Look at the most
recent freshman class and break out high school grade-point
averages by gender. To win admission at many private
colleges (and some publics willing to risk lawsuits),
females had to be more academically adept than
males.
Colleges are about to be called
out for these admissions preferences that discriminate
against women and mask the problem of boys falling behind in
school. In November, the U.S. Commission on Civil Rights
announced an investigation into the practice. Although the
commission lacks the legal authority to act on the
discrimination, mere exposuretriggering outrage from
high school girlsmay force colleges to curtail the
favoritism.
What happens to these less-qualified
males once theyre in college? Many continue their
slack habits from high school, explaining much of the gender
gap in college persistence rates, which count those who earn
degrees within six years.
SMB: Race, sex, and income
issues interact in complicated ways. NAEP (National
Assessment of Educational Progress) data indicate that
income and race gaps are larger than gender gaps in reading
and in math scores at 8th grade, and this pattern holds for
other comparisons as well. In fact, socioeconomic status has
long been the single best predictor of educational
success.
Teachers may be encouraging all
students to continue their education beyond high school, but
the message may be heard differently by male and female
students, and moderated by race and income. Recent data from
a study we are doing here at Wellesley Centers for Women
with a large, racially diverse sample of low-income students
in a large urban school district found that 95 percent of
students, both boys and girls, aspired to attend college
when asked in 9th and 10th grade. But if their actual
college enrollment rates are in line with past district
figures, far fewer will enroll in college and the numbers
for young men will be lower than for young women.
Higher male dropout rates are part of
the problem, but the wider range of better paid jobs open to
young men immediately after high school has also been
influential. Enlisting in the military after high school is
an option for both sexes, but more young men than young
women sign up for the armed forces. Many of these recruits
are attracted by the higher education benefits the military
offers. They may not be rejecting postsecondary education,
but rather simply choosing a different pathway.
EN: Isnt the problem more
complex: boys are learning more math and science and girls
are learning more reading?
RW: When you examine state
tests, which are far better than NAEP for measuring gender
gaps because they test every student every year in most
grades, you see that girls have pulled even with boys in
math and science. In some cases, they outscore boys in those
subjects. At the same time, you see wide gaps in reading and
very wide gaps in writing.
Havent boys always lagged behind
girls in literacy skills? Yes, but literacy skills never
mattered so much as they do today. In 1989 the nations
governors met in Charlottesville, Virginia, to launch the
school reforms we see today. Essentially, the goal was to
put as many students as possible on a college preparation
track. The key tools needed to succeed in college courses,
arts or sciences, are the abilities to read quickly and
accurately and write with precision and accuracy. The
governors were right to set that goal, and educators were
right to respond by teaching those skills in kindergarten
and 1st grade. The problem arose when nobody realized that
boys are ill-equipped to acquire those skills that early, at
least not with the teaching methods used in the past. As a
result, too many boys fall behind, conclude that school is
for girls, and never try to catch up. Once boys shift their
attention to video games or hip-hop music, parents and
educators erroneously conclude those factors trigger the
problem. In fact, boys bury themselves in games after seeing
few rewards for them in school.
Educators havent even started
redesigning the early grades to help boys absorb early
literacy skills. Why this is not happening is unclear. Why
has the Department of Education refused to launch a single
research project into boys academic problems? The most
likely answer: at a time when men rule the White House and
Wall Street, helping males, including young boys, would
amount to a political correctness violation.
SMB: I differ with Richard on
NAEP. NAEP tests are specifically designed to produce
reliable, comparable data over time. State tests are not.
And the NAEP data are clear, if not as dramatic as some
selected state data: boys, on average, perform less well
than girls on tests of reading and writing skills and
low-income boys do less well than higher-income boys. NAEP
data also show that the gaps favor boys in science and math.
While smaller than those favoring girls in reading, the gaps
have by no means disappeared and they grow larger as
students age
Despite widespread concern about
boys literacy skills, we rarely look seriously at the
lingering gender stereotypes that play out every day in our
schools, homes, and communities. As Richard indicates,
gendered assumptions about literacy are at the heart of the
problem, in much the same ways that gendered assumptions
about science and math have inhibited girls
persistence and achievement in these areas. Its a
girl thing to read; real boys dont sit
around with a book. Parenting practices contribute to this;
from an early age mothers read more to their children than
do fathers. In fact, as Lise Eliot delineates in her new
book, Pink Brain, Blue Brain, the way people interact with
babies is based on assumptions about gender differences that
have little basis in biology, but are part and parcel of our
earliest socialization. Little boys need more physical
activity, little girls are more social,
boys are better at math than girlsthe
dichotomies are endless, and they are as dangerous as they
are baseless.
Girls who do what boys have
traditionally done, who become astronauts, scientists,
firefighters, or soldiers, are doing things that almost
everyone sees as moving up. The reverse is not
true. It is no longer legal to advertise job openings under
female or male headings, but our
culture still tends to classify many jobs this way. Women
make up 83 percent of librarians and 92 percent of nurses;
only 15 of the Fortune 500 companies are headed by female
CEOs; and women hold only 17 of 100 seats in the U.S.
Senate.
Gender expectations limit both boys
and girls, and at this point they may constrain young boys
even more than they do girls. One of the most damaging
expectations is that doing well in school is for girls.
Until we confront the reality that many boys fear being
viewed as less than all boy when achieving
academically, we will only be playing around the edges of
the problem.
EN: Are the problems more
apparent in elementary or secondary schooling? Are there
particular subjects or activities where boys are faring
especially well or especially poorly?
RW: In general, girls arrive in
kindergarten far more ready than boys to engage the
verbal-rich curriculum that awaits them. By the end of
elementary school, the gaps become significant, and in
middle school they widen, in part because many schools
dont teach literacy skills after 6th grade, only
literature. In 9th grade, where poorly prepared
boys first encounter the full force of the college-readiness
curriculum, you can see a pileup, or bulge, as 9th-grade
classes are far larger than 8th-grade classes, the result of
students being retained before entering 10th
grade.
Nationally, there are 113 boys in 9th
grade for every 100 girls, according to the Southern
Regional Education Board. Among African Americans, there are
123 boys for every 100 girls. States are discovering that
9th grade has become their biggest dropout year. By 11th
grade many boys begin to revive academically, but its
too late to recover from their poorer grades in 9th and 10th
grades.
Gender gaps are not an issue that can
be easily sorted out by subject. High school girls
outperform boys in many of the Advanced Placement subjects,
including many of the sciences. The exceptions are physics
and computer science, where boys tend to do better. Skeptics
of the boy troubles point to SAT scores, where
males outperform females, without acknowledging the gender
imbalances in the test-takers: far more poor and minority
girls than boys take that test.
SMB: The differences between
boys and girls as they enter school have been vastly
exaggerated. Yes, girls, on average, are more verbally adept
at age five, but this difference is not particularly large,
and many young boys are as ready to read as the girls
sitting next to them. Often lost in the discussion of
girls advantages is the reality that boys outperform
girls on tests of visual and spatial abilities, and do at
least as well on tests of mathematical skills at this age,
and these differences widen as they advance in
school.
However, on measures of fine motor
skills and self-control, girls usually perform better than
boys, and these skills clearly contribute to early school
success. Classrooms that use manipulative materials to
practice spatial skills are as necessary as those that give
special attention to literacy skills for students in need of
help in that area. Literacy is critical and boys need
encouragement and support, but this does not mean that all
girls are fine readers and it certainly does not mean that
gaps in science and math that show girls at a disadvantage
should be dismissed. When more than 75 percent of
undergraduate degrees in the highly paid fields of computer
science and engineering are awarded to young men, the
majority of them white, the idea that we no longer need
focus on these issues for girls and for students of color
does not hold up.
Looking carefully at the gendered
assumptions that underlie our education system gives us a
clearer picture not only of the problems confronting boys in
attaining competencies in reading and writing, but of a
range of school problems that include gender violence, the
continuing imbalance favoring boys in school athletics, and
the over-referral of boysparticularly boys of
colorand the under-referral of girls, to special
education programs. Each of these issues reflects
assumptions about the appropriate roles of men
and women. No discussion of educational equity can ignore
the rising rates of dating violence, sexual harassment, and
bullying in our schools. When young men and boys think that
it is acceptable to verbally harass or physically attack
girls under the guise of manliness, something is
decidedly out of kilter. Educators must do more to help both
boys and girls see beyond this dangerous construction of
masculinity.
EN: Do boys learn differently
than girls? Are schools better organized for the ways in
which girls learn? Or is the problem something in American
culture writ large?
RW: This is not an American
issue. In England and Australia, the gender gap is a topic
of regular newspaper stories. Whats interesting in
England is the attention paid to the especially sharp
decline in educational performance among white boys from
blue-collar families. You can see that in this country as
well, with steeply growing college-going gender gaps within
that group. The issue in Australia came to a head in 2003
when the government issued a lengthy report on the topic.
The conclusion: literacy skills are the culprit. Researchers
in England have reached roughly the same
conclusion.
In the United States the federal
government has never investigated the issue, most likely
because it is considered controversial. When the
issue arises, the basic premise that boys are in trouble
gets attacked by national feminist groups or professors from
womens studies departments. Their attitude is
understandable: the first to point out that boys were in
trouble were conservatives, who blamed the feminists for
creating school environments that were hostile to boys. I
find no evidence that feminists are to blame for the
problem. Their only fault lies in continuing to
deny that the problem exists.
SMB: Different children learn
differently, but differences between individual boys and
between individual girls are much larger than those between
girls as a group and boys as a group. Expectations based on
gender remain rampant in American culture, and indeed, in
cultures around the world. As Richard notes, there has been
significant attention paid to the boy half of gender issues
in England and Australia. Researchers in England who have
studied a range of sociocultural approaches to the problem
of boys achievement report that one of the most
successful involves directly addressing the lad
culture. By helping boys who are seen as leaders in
their peer group improve in school, they create a climate
where other boys see academic achievement as
cool. Exam grades for boys in schools in the
study increased significantly.
Creating an environment where academic
achievement is seen as something all boys, as well as all
girls, should aspire to is critical. In those U.S. school
systems where boys do well, this is invariably the case. The
majority of these schools are in more affluent districts,
where parents have college degrees and encourage their sons
and their daughters to do well academically, or in less
advantaged communities where the community itself has
rallied behind educational goals. The culture of the school
reflects the culture of the surrounding community. We need
more public discussion of the value of education and its
multiple individual and societal benefits. When we talk only
of test scores and economic rewards, we present too narrow a
view.
EN: Is it a problem that so few
teachers are men?
RW: Male teachers continue to
disappear from classrooms. Their numbers are at 24 percent,
a record low. Whats interesting is the rapid
disappearance of male teachers from the middle school
classrooms. Elementary schools never had many male teachers
and high schools still retain a respectable number of males.
In some middle schools, however, you simply wont find
a male teacher. Combine that with the fact that middle
school is the time when the gender gaps widen the most and
you have an obvious culprit, right? I dont buy it. It
wouldnt hurt to have more male teachers, especially in
the middle school years, but Im not convinced that
suddenly boosting the number of male teachers would close
any gender gaps.
Some researchers (see The Why
Chromosome, research, Fall 2006) have documented
modest gains made by boys taught by male teachers, but in
researching my book I found that the schools that educate
boys as well as girls pay little or no attention to the
gender of the teacher. Rather, they pour enormous resources
into how literacy is taught.
SMB: It is not surprising that
there are so few male teachers. K12 teaching remains a
womans job, with a limited career path and
poor pay considering the preparation required. Questions
laced with homophobia about why a man would want to teach
children are rampant. The more advanced the education level,
the more men in the teaching ranks. At the university level
the balance has shifted entirely, with women significantly
underrepresented among tenured faculty. Excellent teaching
is not a matter of gender, but the absence of men in
K12 classrooms sends subtle messages about what is
female and male, influencing
students in ways that remain largely invisible and
understudied.
EN: Is single-sex education a
viable strategy for addressing the problem?
SMB: Research that examines the
effectiveness of single-sex K12 education and controls
for socioeconomic background and degree of parental
involvement, both crucial factors in educational attainment,
is woefully lacking. We must examine curricular programs and
teaching practices used in successful single-sex and coed
programs, the kinds of students they help most, and how
these programs and practices can work for more students in a
wider range of settings. An example of this approach is
research showing that girls benefit from science instruction
that relates the material to real-world problemsand so
do boys. When evaluating single-sex education,
we must not ignore a crucial purpose
of public educationdeveloping effective citizens. We
need to consider the tradeoffs we may be making in
sex-segregating students, closing off opportunities for
learning from and with each other.
RW: Heres my problem with
single-sex education: The Bush Department of Education
flipped on the green light for public schools to carry out
single-sex education, but never commissioned a single study
that would instruct schools on how to do it. (Im
choosing my words carefully here: meta-analyses of
single-sex education dont guide classroom
instruction.) Some statesSouth Carolina comes to mind,
which was determined to do something for their flailing
boysgave that green light a broad embrace, unleashing
several hundred programs. Unfortunately, not that many of
those programs are first-rate. And if academic breakthroughs
dont materialize, those single-sex programs will be
dismantled, perhaps prematurely.
EN: Are there programs that are
much more effective for boys? What are the traits or
approaches that they have in common?
RW: Most important is a refusal
to let students slip behind. I see a lax attitude toward
males, Dont worry, Mom, boys will be boys. Your
son will catch up, as the single biggest problem. In
fact, a lot of boys never do catch up. Two of the schools I
profile in Why Boys Fail werent even aware they were
closing gender gaps; that wasnt their goal. Their goal
was to focus on literacy skills and refuse to let any child
slip behind. They took great pride in their success and
seemed surprised when it was pointed out they had leveled
the gender gaps.
SMB: Research studies on
effective schools have shown remarkably similar findings for
30 years. Schools that set high standards for all, involve
parents, provide firm discipline and an orderly, encouraging
environment, and where teachers are respected and engaged
are more successful. Such schools do not as easily fall into
the black hole of differential expectations for girls and
boys, or one racial or ethnic group over another.
EN: What other options might
policymakers or reformers consider?
SMB: We should take a page from
the successful, ongoing efforts that address the lingering
lag in girls and womens participation in STEM
(Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics) fields
and leadership positions: 1) mentoring and role-modeling
programs that involve more men in schools, particularly men
who hold other than traditionally male jobs so that students
see men in a variety of careers; 2) a national fathers
reading campaign to engage more fathers in reading to their
children; and 3) increased funding for innovative programs
that engage students in literacy activities in and out of
school. When reading like a girl is as
acceptable for boys as doing science and math well is
becoming for girls, we will begin to make real progress
toward gender-equitable education for all our
students.
RW: The U.S. Department of
Education needs to launch an Australian-style investigation
into the boys problem. Once the key issues are identified,
follow-on research projects can target specific teaching
strategies for teachers. One critical need: national
research into what works and doesnt work with
single-sex education.
Not all the solutions lie within the
K12 world, however. Colleges should eliminate from
high school grade-point averages the results from 9th
gradewhen many boys struggle to make the transition
from middle school. And colleges need to step in to help
make badly needed adjustments to K12 accountability
systems. State high school graduation standards dont
match college readiness requirements. Given the higher
college dropout rates for men, that mismatch appears to be
hurting males the most.
Source: educationnext.org/gender-gap/
Girls Make
Higher Grades than Boys in All School Subjects, Analysis
Finds
Despite the stereotype that boys do better in math and
science, girls have made higher grades than boys throughout
their school years for nearly a century, according to a new
analysis published by the American Psychological
Association.
Although gender differences
follow essentially stereotypical patterns on achievement
tests in which boys typically score higher on math and
science, females have the advantage on school grades
regardless of the material, said lead study author
Daniel Voyer, PhD, of the University of New Brunswick,
Fredericton, Canada. School marks reflect learning in
the larger social context of the classroom and require
effort and persistence over long periods of time, whereas
standardized tests assess basic or specialized academic
abilities and aptitudes at one point in time without social
influences.
Based on research from 1914 through
2011 that spanned more than 30 countries, the study found
the differences in grades between girls and boys were
largest for language courses and smallest for math and
science. The female advantage in school performance in math
and science did not become apparent until junior or middle
school, according to the study, published in the APA journal
Psychological Bulletin®. The degree of gender difference
in grades increased from elementary to middle school, but
decreased between high school and college.
The researchers examined 369 samples
from 308 studies, reflecting grades of 538,710 boys and
595,332 girls. Seventy percent of the samples consisted of
students from the United States. Other countries or regions
represented by more than one sample included Norway, Canada,
Turkey, Germany, Taiwan, Malaysia, Israel, New Zealand,
Australia, Sweden, Slovakia, United Kingdom, Africa and
Finland. Countries represented by one sample included
Belgium, Czech Republic, Estonia, Mexico, Hong Kong, India,
Iran, Jordan, the Netherlands, Portugal, Saudi Arabia,
Serbia and Slovenia.
All studies included an evaluation of
gender differences in teacher-assigned grades or official
grade point averages in elementary, junior/middle or high
school, or undergraduate and graduate university. Studies
that relied on self-report and those about special
populations, such as high-risk or mentored students, were
excluded. The studies also looked at variables that might
affect the students grades, such as the country where
students attended school, course material, students
ages at the time the grades were obtained, the study date
and racial composition of the samples.
The study reveals that recent claims
of a boy crisis, with boys lagging behind girls
in school achievement, are not accurate because girls
grades have been consistently higher than boys across
several decades with no significant changes in recent years,
the authors wrote.
The fact that females generally
perform better than their male counterparts throughout what
is essentially mandatory schooling in most countries seems
to be a well-kept secret, considering how little attention
it has received as a global phenomenon, said co-author
Susan Voyer, MASc, also of the University of New
Brunswick.
As for why girls perform better in
school than boys, the authors speculated that social and
cultural factors could be among several possible
explanations. Parents may assume boys are better at math and
science so they might encourage girls to put more effort
into their studies, which could lead to the slight advantage
girls have in all courses, they wrote. Gender differences in
learning styles is another possibility. Previous research
has shown girls tend to study in order to understand the
materials, whereas boys emphasize performance, which
indicates a focus on the final grades. Mastery of the
subject matter generally produces better marks than
performance emphasis, so this could account in part for
males lower marks than females, the authors
wrote.
Article: Gender Differences in
Scholastic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis, Daniel Voyer,
PhD, and Susan D. Voyer, MASc, University of New Brunswick,
Psychological Bulletin, online April 28, 2014.
Daniel Voyer can be contacted by email
or by phone at 1-506-453-4974.
The American Psychological
Association, in Washington, D.C., is the largest scientific
and professional organization representing psychology in the
United States. APA's membership includes nearly 130,000
researchers, educators, clinicians, consultants and
students. Through its divisions in 54 subfields of
psychology and affiliations with 60 state, territorial and
Canadian provincial associations, APA works to advance the
creation, communication and application of psychological
knowledge to benefit society and improve people's lives.
Source: www.apa.org/news/press/releases/2014/04/girls-grades.aspx

How do we help
boys close the academic gender gap?
Sit still. Read quietly to yourself. Stop fidgeting. Such
warnings are often directed at boys, but you may as well be
asking them to do advanced calculus.
parenting now logoGirls are more
attentive, more organized and perform better socially and
academically, according to recent research published by the
Third Way, a centrist policy institute. And the gap between
girls and boys exceeds that of any other two
groups.
The social and behavioral skills
gap between boys and girls is considerably larger than the
gap between children from poor families and middle class
families or the gap between black and white children,
the study reads.
Some attribute the growing divide to a
learning environment structured to favor girls.
Girls have become the gold
standard, Michael Thompson, author of Its
a Boy! and Raising Cain: Protecting the
Emotional Life of Boys, told NewsHour. We, as
parents, have decided that we need earlier reading scores.
Then weve made kindergarten the new first grade. There
is more emphasis on learning earlier and earlier. Boys just
arent programmed like that thats obvious
from a physical and psychological
standpoint.
By the 8th grade, 48 percent of girls
receive a mix of A and B grades compared to 31 percent of
boys, the research shows. The gap remains through high
school and in college. Nearly 60 percent of college
graduates are women.
The academic gap takes a psychological
toll on boys too. Boys are far more likely to be suspended
or expelled from school, in fact they account for 71 percent
of all school suspensions according to the U.S. Department
of Education and the Schott Foundation Report.
Boys are also far less likely to ask
for help when they dont understand a
subject.
If you treat girls as the gold
standards and boys as defective girls, thats going to
be demoralizing, Thompson said. What do
elementary and junior high girls always say about boys their
age? You are so immature. If thats the
norm, then this system is just rigged against the
boys.
So, how can parents help turn this
around? We have tips from two experts on how to help boys
keep up with their female counterparts and succeed in
school.
Marie Roker-Jones founded
Raising Great Men and is the senior editor at
The Good Men Project.
- Make sure your home encourages
learning. Have books, learning materials and tools that
support your sons learning style. Create a learning
environment at home that reflects what your son is
learning at school. There needs to be a
school-home-life-connection to make education appealing
to your son.
- Create a safe, learning space in
your home, one that would help make education appealing
to your son. Photo by JGI/Jamie Grill/Blend
Images
- Create a learning space in your
home, one that would help make education appealing to
your son. Photo by JGI/Jamie Grill/Blend
Images
- Set goals for the academic year.
Work with your son at the beginning of the school year to
set realistic academic goals. The goals should be fluid
and adjusted as the school year progresses. Talk about
expectations and plans for achieving academic
success.
- Create a partnership with your
sons school. Work with administrators and teachers
as a team to ensure your sons success. Let teachers
know you are an active partner.
- Create a safe space for your son
to discuss challenges/concerns about school. Have weekly
or monthly check-ins with your son to talk about how what
is going on at school. Use this opportunity to listen
more than speak. Provide guidance rather than
criticism.
- Help him to recognize his
abilities. Focus on your sons strengths and help
him identify areas in need of improvement.
- Bonus: Set guidelines and show him
how to balance work and play time. Be
consistent with helping him manage his time.
Michael Thompson is the author of
Raising Cain: Protecting the Emotional Life of
Boys among other books and is a psychologist
specializing in the emotional development of
boys.
- Fathers need to spend more time
reading to their children both boys and girls
more time helping with homework and more time
attending non-athletic school events. When boys see that
their fathers only come to town sports, they conclude
that school is not of great interest to men and therefore
not a path to manhood. When fathers, even uneducated
fathers, come to school and spend time on schoolwork with
their sons, it has a huge positive impact.
- Fathers can help their son's
development by being involved with the child's
schoolwork, reading and other non-athletic activities.
Photo by Cavan Images
- Fathers can help their sons
development by being involved with the childs
schoolwork, reading and supporting other non-athletic
activities. Photo by Cavan Images
-
- Parents need to allow their
children to engage in free, undirected outdoor play as
much as possible. When boys organize their own groups,
play their own games and have their own adventures, it
makes them more competent and confident, readier to
tackle tasks in the more constrained world of
school.
- Teachers need to be taught that in
the classroom boys respond favorably to lessons that
involve movement, teamwork, competition, a public product
(producing a video clip, reciting a poem or lines from a
play) and some psychological hook: humor, a mystery, a
puzzle. The steady diet of a quiet classroom with an
emphasis on individual reading or paper-andpencil
work is designed to make boys feels they are in
jail.
- Schools are constantly interfering
with boys play at recess and are constantly banning
the kinds of stories they like to write in an
un-scientific effort at violence prevention. There is no
scientific basis for banning boys play or their
so-called violent stories just because they
are not to the teachers taste.
We must all recognize that boys, even
more than girls, are relational learners. They only work
hard for teachers who are interested in them as people, who
are curious about a boys life outside school, and who
have a sense of humor about some oppositional behavior on
the part of boys. Boys work hard for teachers who trust boy
development.
Source: www.pbs.org/newshour/updates/classroom-rigged-boys/#

The Reverse
Gender Gap
he data is indisputable: Boys now lag behind girls in
several significant areas of education. But the roots of the
new gender gap are more complex and nuanced than has been
reported. And so are the solutions.
For years, women lagged behind men in
educational attainment. More boys went to college, and
Census data shows that twice as many men as women got
bachelors degrees in 1960. Two decades later, a funny
thing began to happen. By the mid-1980s, women not only
caught up but also started to gain on men, not just by
inches, but miles. Now, 57 percent of college students are
women, and women earn about one-third more bachelors
degrees than men, says the National Center for Education
Statistics.
As attention focuses on girls and
women in STEM fields and they skyrocket ahead, however, some
argue that the boys and men are getting left behind. In
fact, a Mt. Everest of evidence points to an entrenched
reverse gender gapboys lagging behind girls that
surfaces as early as kindergarten. Heres a sample:
According to U.S. Department of Education data, boys receive
71 percent of school suspensions. Boys make up 67 percent of
special education classrooms. Boys are five times more
likely than girls to be labeled hyperactive and 30 percent
more likely to flunk or drop out of school.
Its a story about
females real gains, but also about stagnation in
education for males that raises daunting challenges for
American society, write sociologists Thomas A. DiPrete
and Claudia Buchmann in their new book The Rise of Women:
The Growing Gender Gap in Education and What It Means for
American Schools.
One of the most talked about issues in
education, the widening achievement difference between boys
and girls has been debated vociferously for more than a
decade. In the popular press, four words encapsulate the
crux of the matter: Are schools failing boys?
The question has a way of raising
hackles and attracting polarizing expositions on the state
of American schooling that are often fraught with political
agendas. It suggests that there is a war against boys, as
author Christina Hoff Sommers argued in her 2001 book of
that title, and that gains by girls have been at the expense
of boys. Other experts reject framing the conversation in
terms of winners and losers. One of those is Adam J. Cox, a
clinical psychologist who earned his doctorate in counseling
psychology at Lehigh University. He has written extensively
on the social and emotional development of children and
adolescents based on his experiences in his private
practices in Pennsylvania and, more recently, Rhode
Island.
The anxiety that lies beneath
much of the gender consternation is a zero sum mentality
that leads some to believe that if we devote resources and
attention to boys, those resources and that attention must
result in a concomitant reduction for girls, says Cox,
author of the 2006 Boys of Few Words: Raising Our Sons to
Communicate and Connect and the 2012 On Purpose Before
Twenty. I think thats
erroneous.
While the data exposes an indisputable
gender gap, the reasons why this is the case are anything
but settled. Some researchers point to biological
differences between boys and girls in the way they learn.
Others say the gap all but dissolves when race and
socio-economic factors are considered. Some advocate
single-gender schools; others see little value, and perhaps
even disadvantage, in that approach.
Some writers even argue that the
gender gap has not truly resulted in any disadvantage to
boys in the long run. They point to a wage gap that
persists, with women earning on average only 77 cents to
every $1 men get, according to Census data. Men also run the
vast majority of Fortune 500 companies; women hold only 4.2
percent of those CEO positions, according to the nonprofit
Catalyst. And women are under-represented at all levels of
government. For example, women hold only 18 percent of the
seats in Congress, says the Center for American Women and
Politics.
However, Hoff Sommers contends, those
data have little to do with educational attainment in school
and instead are often used as a way to distract people from
the very real effects of the three decade- long focus on
girls and their needs. In reality, there is not a single
explanation or fix, as is often the case.
The roots of the gender gap are much
more complex and nuanced than often reported. Many factors
can account for the discrepancies between the academic
achievement of boys and girls. It is a picture that
continues to emerge, and most recently, has focused on
social norms for boys and girls and the way those standards
impact educational success.
In Boys of Few Words, Cox argues that
many boys face communication challengesborn of innate
brain differences and learning styles as well as social
pressures and stereotypesthat hurt their chances of
success in traditional classrooms, and ultimately the
workplace, now more than ever.
In your sons
twenty-first-century education, career, and relationships,
hell be expected to participate in highly social
networks, he writes in his book. His success
will hinge on how well he can access and join those
networks. According to Cox, boys often feel alienated
in school from the earliest grades. They feel as
though its a place they dont belong, where their
particular ways of processing are not valued, he says.
Very often, were pathologizing boys for being
boys. They are being treated as less-than members of a
classroom, and people who are deficient or insufficient in a
number of ways. The key, Cox says, is to teach boys at
young ages strategies to connect and communicate, to build
their communication competency. That might involve learning
how to give compliments or cultivating a conscience. In
schools, Cox advocates social-skills groups, where teachers
and students have conversations and students learn to voice
their opinions.
This is a huge social
issue, he says. We are so much more of a social
culture. It seems your success in the world, whether
youre accepted and liked by other people, the degree
to which others see you as smart and successfula lot
of that has to do with your ability to read subtle social
signals. It is important that boys be oriented to those
things at a time when their brain still has neuroplasticity,
when there is still learning going on. To be fair,
many boys excel in traditional schools,
and there is more variation among boys
and girls than between the genders. Researchers also agree
that the overall academic prowess of girls is not due to
more smarts. In fact, boys and girls share very similar
cognitive abilities.
In non-cognitive skills, however,
differences are significant. A 2012 study by Christopher
Cornwell, head of the economics department at the University
of Georgia, and colleagues found that boys on average score
15 percent lower on an assessment of non-cognitive skills
(engagement in class, ability to sit calmly, interpersonal
skills) than girls. The study falls short of calling
teachers sexist, but points to the fact that the majority of
elementary teachers are female, for the first time
suggesting that a gender gap persists as a function of
educators behavioral perceptions of their
students.
In addition, more girls like their
teachers and schools. According to 2007 data from the Early
Childhood Longitudinal Survey (ECLS-K), 67 percent of
eighth-grade girls report enjoying school compared to 59
percent of eighth-grade boys.
The difference in engagement, in
particular, could explain the gender gap. Interestingly,
boys tend to start school as eager and as excited as girls,
often with similar connectedness to teachers, says James
Earl Davis, a professor of educational leadership at Temple
University in Philadelphia who studies gender and
education.
Then its squelched early
on, by third or fourth grade. Why the change? Boys
continue to get the message theyre not doing
right, Davis says, echoing Cox.
The Cornwell study also found that
primary school teachers generally graded boys lower than
girls, even though the boys scored similarly or better than
the girls on standardized tests. For example, the data show
that although boys out perform girls on math and science
test scores, girls are assigned the higher grades by their
teachers. The misalignment, the researchers say, is because
teachers factored behavior and comportment into the
mix.
Besides social norms, some observers
have attributed boys lower interest in school to
biological differences. Cox, for example, points out that
girls in general interpret language better than boys, an
advantage that can carry over to the classroom. Girls
hear language more deeply, using more of their brain to
process and understand language than do boys, he
writes. The expanded processing capability adds up to
additional social perception and the versatility to use that
knowledge in social communication.
Learning styles, in general, also vary
between boys and girls. While many girls absorb academic
lessons by listening and looking, many boys rely on
kinesthetic learning, that is movement and touch, to master
new information, Cox says. The typical classroom, however,
rarely involves moving around. In fact, students are
expected to sit still in rows of desks while the teacher
delivers the lesson.
Many kinesthetic-oriented boys are the
ones getting in trouble at school for fidgeting and worse.
At an all-boys school, a lot of that is let go,
Cox says. People dont constantly provide a
correction. At the Fenn School, an independent, 4th to
9th-grade boys school in Concord, Mass., the Lower School
day includes flop, a few minutes break to
just flop on the ground, Cox explains.
That is an ideal intervention for boys. Its
highly effective in helping them to cope with restlessness
and excessive energy.
He also urges more male teachers in
the lower grades to serve as role models, more boy-friendly
books (sci-fi, action and adventure) and more projects that
involve doing something heroic.
Where is the room to be Harry
Potter? he asks. The answer could be as simple as
allowing older students to mentor younger ones, he says.
While research shows that male teachers in the lower grades
improve the likability of school for boys, it does not
necessarily translate into stronger academic outcomes.
All-boys schools also have mixed results. Theres
been a proliferation of same-sex schools and classrooms in
public schooling, Davis says. We would love to
see more achievement outcomes. He reminds that urban
schools, in particular, struggle with issues of teacher
competencyand that issue does not go away simply by
reorganizing the students into single sex groups. DiPrete,
who also is the Giddings professor of sociology at Columbia
University in New York, says his research shows that the
gender gap diminishes in relation to the strength of the
academic climate in a schooland that often depends
upon the girls. Girls, as a group, tend to be more
academic-oriented, he says. When you take all
the girls out, you are simultaneously, in general, making
the classroom environment less academic. And that hurts
boys.
Still, many schools are seeing
positive results from single-sex options. In South Carolina,
for example, more than 100 co-ed public schools are
experimenting with segregating boys and girls for large
chunks of the school day when core subjects are taught.
Overall, reports suggest increased academic performance and
decreased disciplinary issues for boys and girls in these
single-gender classes.
There should always be
opportunities for girls to be among girls and for boys to be
among boys, and hopefully for girls to be nurtured by women
and boys nurtured by men, says Davis, who likes the
idea of co-ed schools that use gender pullouts for
academics, not just to address social and health
issues.
However, the opportunities for boys to
be exposed to male role models are distinctively
disadvantaged across the nation by the dominance of women in
teaching. Some have posited, not without warrant, that
female teachers instinctively reinforce female
behavior and fail to acknowledge, or even punish, the
gender-specific behaviors of boys.
Peter J. Kuriloff, research director
of the Center for the Study of Boys and Girls
Lives in Philadelphia and a professor of teaching, learning
and leadership at the University of Pennsylvania, argues
that public schools in general are failing both boys and
girls. The way the kids are taught in most ordinary
public schools is boring, he says. Thats
not good for boys or girls.
DiPrete says schools have not kept
pace with a changing economy and that boys have suffered
more for it. The economy has changed
fundamentally, he says, as manufacturing jobs have
given way to an information age that requires higher
education. Boys as a group are still operating in this
old world that doesnt exist anymore. Girls have made a
more successful transition.
Schools, he says, need to help all
students, but particularly boys, see clearer pathways, the
practical value of education as a way to improve
engagement.
Cox would agree. He has spent two
years interviewing boys from around the world, the basis of
On Purpose Before Twenty. We cannot hope to educate
the next generation for the good life, he writes,
without making a more considered life part of that
equation. Schools are an essential catalyst for this growth,
and for shaping people whose strength should be manifest
more in their citizenship than consumption. Because
the education of youth has become unbalanced, he adds,
favoring an intake of content over a plan of action,
young people are searching for a sense of
agency.
Those words resonate with Alexander
Kopelman, co-founder and head of the Childrens Arts
Guild in New York. The after-school, nonprofit program seeks
to help boys develop emotional skills by way of art.We
feel gender socialization is very detrimental to boys,
he says, noting the gender gap. Boys and men, he says, are
often taught to swallow their feelings. Consider the
classic, boys dont cry. What were learning
is to cut away a very essential part of ourselves, and to
view it as the other, and as something thats weak and
undesirable, he says. As we get older, we sort
ourselves into what boys and girls do and dont
do.
That can impact interest in
booksand ultimately academic performance. Or it can
impact love of artand creative expression. Many boys,
Kopelman says, will not pursue art because its viewed
as something girls do. At the guild, the goal is to break
down those stereotypes. Boys aged pre-K through fifth grade
are exposed to male role models and creative projects. They
might have a contest to see which team can build the tallest
structure, or they might cook together or draw with
pastels.
The fundamental question, he says,
should not be focused on whether schools are failing boys.
That only stirs hard feelings and controversy, he says.
Others posit that it was exactly this type of focus (i.e.,
are schools failing girls?) that resulted in the
impressive gains made by girls over the past several years
and should be used as a model for boys. Conservative author
Kate OBeirne contends the past two decades have seen
many classrooms turn into reeducation camps for young
boys.
Kopelman asks what is the practical
value of an education and how should that education look.
What, he says, are we preparing students
for? The answer when it comes to boys may well demand
schools of the future that look quite different from
current, traditional models.<
As Cox says: We are a diverse
species, and we have a range of differences. We ought to
have schools, which are so important, that accommodate some
of those differences.
Source: coe.lehigh.edu/content/reverse-gender-gap
Social
Networks Influence Academic Performance: Study
Social networks influence academic performance among
students, finds a new study.
Students tend to perform better with
high-performers among their friends, as some people are
capable of inspiring others to try harder, according to the
researchers from the HSE Centre for Institutional Studies in
Russia.
The recent studies indicate that the
role of the social environment may be underestimated, as
classmates can greatly influence one another's behaviour and
academic success.
Using 2013-2023 data on the social
networks of 117 first-year students examined whether
students consider academic success in choosing friends among
their classmates and whether friends influence each other's
academic performance.
Students do not usually consider
academic performance, but over time - often in the middle of
the academic year - all members in a peer group tend to
perform at about the same level.
Thus, most students who surrounded
themselves with high-achievers improved their performance
over time. The opposite was also true - those who befriended
underachievers eventually experienced a drop in
grades.
According to the authors, while
underachievers have a stronger influence on their networks,
high performers tend to gain popularity and expand their
influence over time, particularly by helping other students
with their studies.
Men were found to have larger networks
than women, and all students were more likely to be friends
with those whom they had known before college, classmates of
the same gender, and members of their study
group.
The findings
were published in the journal Educational Studies.
Source: gadgets.ndtv.com/social-networking/news/social-networks-influence-academic-performance-study-776376
Girls lead
boys in academic achievement globally
Geary determined that girls outperform boys in educational
achievement in 70 percent of the countries they studied,
regardless of the level of gender, political, economic or
social equality.
Considerable attention has been paid
to how boys' educational achievements in science and math
compare to girls' accomplishments in those areas, often
leading to the assumption that boys outperform girls in
these areas. Now, using international data, researchers at
the University of Missouri and the University of Glasgow in
Glasgow, Scotland, have determined that girls outperform
boys in educational achievement in 70 percent of the
countries they studiedregardless of the level of
gender, political, economic or social equality.
"We studied the educational
achievement levels of 1.5 million 15-year-olds from around
the world using data collected between 2000 and 2010," said
David Geary, Curators Professor of Psychological Sciences in
the College of Arts and Science at MU. "Even in countries
where women's liberties are severely restricted, we found
that girls are outperforming boys in reading, mathematics,
and science literacy by age 15, regardless of political,
economic, social or gender equality issues and policies
found in those countries."
According to the data, boys fall
behind girls in overall achievement across reading,
mathematics, and science in 70 percent of the countries
studied. Boys outperform girls in only three countries or
regions: Colombia, Costa Rica and the Indian state, Himachal
Pradesh. Boys and girls had similar educational achievements
in the United States and United Kingdom.
In countries known for relatively low
gender equality ratings, such as Qatar, Jordan and the
United Arab Emirates, the educational achievement gap is
relatively large and favors girls.
The one exception worldwide is among
students in economically developed nations where high
achieving boys outperform high achieving girls, researchers
said.
"With the exception of high-achievers,
boys have poorer educational outcomes than girls around the
world, independent of social equality indicators," said
Gijsbert Stoet, reader in psychology at the University of
Glasgow. "Results show that a commitment to gender equality
on its own is not enough to close the achievement gaps in
global education; the gap is not increasing. Although it is
vital that we promote gender equality in schools, we also
need to make sure that we're doing more to understand why
these gaps, especially among boys, persist and what other
policies we can develop to close them."
The study also has important
implications for educational policy, the researchers
said.
"The data will influence how
policymakers think about the options available," said Geary.
"For example, to increase levels of equal opportunities in
education. We believe that policymakers and educators should
not expect that broad progress in social equality will
necessarily result in educational equality. In fact, we
found that with the exception of high achievers, boys have
poorer educational outcomes than girls around the world,
independent of social equality indicators. Therefore, in
order to effectively close the gaps in achievement,
education policymakers should consider factors other than
political, economic and social equality, and especially as
related to boys' overall achievement and high-achieving
girls' interest in mathematics and science."
The study, "Sex differences in
academic achievement are not related to political, economic
or social equality," recently was published in the peer
reviewed journal, Intelligence.
Source: phys.org/news/2015-01-girls-boys-academic-globally.html
Why
Girls Tend to Get Better Grades Than Boys Do
New research shows that girls are ahead in every
subject, including math and science. Do today's grading
methods skew in their favor?
You can help shape The Atlantics
futureand itll only take you 10-15 minutes. Tell
us what you think in our 2016 Atlantic Audience
Survey.
As the new school year ramps up,
teachers and parents need to be reminded of a well-kept
secret: Across all grade levels and academic subjects, girls
earn higher grades than boys. Not just in the United States,
but across the globe, in countries as far afield as Norway
and Hong Kong.
This finding is reflected in a recent
study by psychology professors Daniel and Susan Voyer at the
University of New Brunswick. The Voyers based their results
on a meta-analysis of 369 studies involving the academic
grades of over one million boys and girls from 30 different
nations. The findings are unquestionably robust: Girls earn
higher grades in every subject, including the
science-related fields where boys are thought to surpass
them.
Less of a secret is the gender
disparity in college enrollment rates. The latest data from
the Pew Research Center uses U.S. Census Bureau data to show
that in 2012, 71 percent of female high school graduates
went on to college, compared to 61 percent of their male
counterparts. In 1994 the figures were 63 and 61 percent,
respectively. In other words, college enrollment rates for
young women are climbing while those of young men remain
flat.
This begs a sensitive question: Are
schools set up to favor the way girls learn and trip up
boys?
Girls succeed over
boys in school because they are more apt to plan ahead,
set academic goals, and put effort into achieving those
goals.
Lets start with kindergarten.
Claire Cameron from the Center for the Advanced Study of
Teaching and Learning at the University of Virginia has
dedicated her career to studying kindergarten readiness in
kids. Shes found that little ones who are destined to
do well in a typical 21st century kindergarten class are
those who manifest good self-regulation. This is a term that
is bandied about a great deal these days by teachers and
psychologists. It mostly refers to disciplined behaviors
like raising ones hand in class, waiting ones
turn, paying attention, listening to and following
teachers instructions, and restraining oneself from
blurting out answers. These skills are prerequisites for
most academically oriented kindergarten classes in
Americaas well as basic prerequisites for success in
life.
As it turns out, kindergarten-age
girls have far better self-regulation than boys. A few years
ago, Cameron and her colleagues confirmed this by putting
several hundred 5 and 6-year-old boys and girls through a
type of Simon-Says game called the Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders
Task. Trained research assistants rated the kids
ability to follow the correct instruction and not be thrown
off by a confounding onein some cases, for instance,
they were instructed to touch their toes every time they
were asked to touch their heads. Curiously enough,
remembering such rules as touch your head really means
touch your toes and inhibiting the urge to touch
ones head instead amounts to a nifty example of good
overall self-regulation.
The researchers combined the results
of boys and girls scores on the
Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Task with parents and
teachers ratings of these same kids capacity to
pay attention, follow directions, finish schoolwork, and
stay organized. The outcome was remarkable. They discovered
that boys were a whole year behind girls in all areas of
self-regulation. By the end of kindergarten, boys were just
beginning to acquire the self-regulatory skills with which
girls had started the year.
This self-discipline edge for girls
carries into middle-school and beyond. In a 2006 landmark
study, Martin Seligman and Angela Lee Duckworth found that
middle-school girls edge out boys in overall
self-discipline. This contributes greatly to their better
grades across all subjects. They found that girls are
more adept at reading test instructions before
proceeding to the questions, paying attention to
a teacher rather than daydreaming, choosing
homework over TV, and persisting on long-term
assignments despite boredom and frustration. These top
cognitive scientists from the University of Pennsylvania
also found that girls are apt to start their homework
earlier in the day than boys and spend almost double the
amount of time completing it. Girls grade point
averages across all subjects were higher than those of boys,
even in basic and advanced mathwhich, again, are seen
as traditional strongholds of boys.
What Drs. Seligman and Duckworth label
self-discipline, other researchers name
conscientiousness. Or, a predisposition to plan
ahead, set goals, and persist in the face of frustrations
and setbacks. Conscientiousness is uniformly considered by
social scientists to be an inborn personality trait that is
not evenly distributed across all humans. In fact, a host of
cross-cultural studies show that females tend to be more
conscientious than males. One such study by Lindsay
Reddington out of Columbia University even found that female
college students are far more likely than males to jot down
detailed notes in class, transcribe what professors say more
accurately, and remember lecture content better. Arguably,
boys less developed conscientiousness leaves them at a
disadvantage in school settings where grades heavily weight
good organizational skills alongside demonstrations of
acquired knowledge.
These days, the whole school
experience seems to play right into most girls
strengthsand most boys weaknesses. Gone are the
days when you could blow off a series of homework
assignments throughout the semester but pull through with a
respectable grade by cramming for and acing that
all-important mid-term exam. Getting good grades today is
far more about keeping up with and producing quality
homeworknot to mention handing it in on
time.
Gwen Kenney-Benson, a psychology
professor at Allegheny College, a liberal arts institution
in Pennsylvania, says that girls succeed over boys in school
because they tend to be more mastery-oriented in their
schoolwork habits. They are more apt to plan ahead, set
academic goals, and put effort into achieving those goals.
They also are more likely than boys to feel intrinsically
satisfied with the whole enterprise of organizing their
work, and more invested in impressing themselves and their
teachers with their efforts.
These days, the
whole school experience seems to play right into most
girls strengthsand most boys
weaknesses.
On the whole, boys approach schoolwork
differently. They are more performance-oriented. Studying
for and taking tests taps into their competitive instincts.
For many boys, tests are quests that get their hearts
pounding. Doing well on them is a public demonstration of
excellence and an occasion for a high-five. In contrast,
Kenney-Benson and some fellow academics provide evidence
that the stress many girls experience in test situations can
artificially lower their performance, giving a false reading
of their true abilities. These researchers arrive at the
following overarching conclusion: The testing
situation may underestimate girls abilities, but the
classroom may underestimate boys
abilities.
It is easy to for boys to feel
alienated in an environment where homework and organization
skills account for so much of their grades. But the
educational tide may be turning in small ways that give boys
more of a fighting chance. An example of this is what
occurred several years ago at Ellis Middle School, in
Austin, Minnesota. Teachers realized that a sizable chunk of
kids who aced tests trundled along each year getting
Cs, Ds, and Fs. At the same time, about 10
percent of the students who consistently obtained As
and Bs did poorly on important tests. Grading policies
were revamped and school officials smartly decided to
furnish kids with two separate grades each semester. One
grade was given for good work habits and citizenship, which
they called a life skills grade. A
knowledge grade was given based on average
scores across important tests. Tests could be retaken at any
point in the semester, provided a student was up to date on
homework.
Staff at Ellis Middle School also
stopped factoring homework into a kids grade. Homework
was framed as practice for tests. Incomplete or tardy
assignments were noted but didnt lower a kids
knowledge grade. The whole enterprise of severely
downgrading kids for such transgressions as occasionally
being late to class, blurting out answers, doodling instead
of taking notes, having a messy backpack, poking the kid in
front, or forgetting to have parents sign a permission slip
for a class trip, was revamped.
It is easy to for
boys to feel alienated in an environment where homework
and organization skills account for so much of their
grades.
This last point was of particular
interest to me. On countless occasions, I have attended
school meetings for boy clients of mine who are in an ADHD
red-zone. I have learned to request a grade print-out in
advance. Not uncommonly, there is a checkered history of
radically different grades: A, A, A, B, B, F, F, A. When F
grades and a resultant zero points are given for late or
missing assignments, a students C grade does not
reflect his academic performance. Since boys tend to be less
conscientious than girlsmore apt to space out and
leave a completed assignment at home, more likely to fail to
turn the page and complete the questions on the backa
distinct fairness issue comes into play when a boys
occasional lapse results in a low grade. Sadly though, it
appears that the overwhelming trend among teachers is to
assign zero points for late work. In one survey by Conni
Campbell, associate dean of the School of Education at Point
Loma Nazarene University, 84 percent of teachers did just
that.
Disaffected boys may also benefit from
a boot camp on test-taking, time-management, and study
habits. These core skills are not always picked up by
osmosis in the classroom, or from diligent parents at home.
Of course, addressing the learning gap between boys and
girls will require parents, teachers and school
administrators to talk more openly about the ways each
gender approaches classroom learningand that
difference itself remains a tender topic.
Source:
theatlantic.com/education/archive/2014/09/why-girls-get-better-grades-than-boys-do/380318/
Why Do More
Women than Men Go to College?
According to the Census Bureau, U.S. women now lead men
in educational attainment for the first time since the
Census began tracking the measure in 1940. The headlines
surprised me: I assumed we had passed that milestone
already, since women have earned more than half of
bachelors degrees each year since the early 1980s.
That women have grown more likely to
graduate from college in the past few decades seems natural:
We have access to a far wider range of careers than
our great-grandmothers did, and the economy favors the
well-educated now more than ever. In light of the rising
value of a college degree, its just strange that men
havent prioritized their education as much as women
have. Why havent they? Why do women increasingly
outnumber men on college campuses?
In a new article in Family Relations,
William Doherty, Brian Willoughby, and Jason Wilde provide
evidence for a thesis not many have considered: Changes in
family structure have contributed to the growing gender gap
in college enrollment. Growing up with stably married
parents makes children of both sexes more likely to succeed
at school, even controlling for socioeconomic status, but
the absence of a father seems to hurt boys more than it does
girls. Thus, as father absence becomes more prevalent, girls
gain a relative advantage in the classroom.
Doherty and his coauthors share two
major findings that support this theory. First, because they
noticed that the college enrollment gender gap . . .
began to emerge about 18 years after the beginning of major
population shifts in family structure, they compared
nonmarital birth rates from 1948 to 1993 with the gender
difference in college enrollment from 1966 to 2011. They
discovered a near-perfect linear relationship
between changes in the two measures: The higher the
nonmarital birthrate grew, the lower the ratio of males to
females [enrolled in college] became as each birth
cohort reached age 18.
Since correlation does not prove
causation, the three researchers then sought to replicate
that basic finding at an individual level. For this
investigation, they used longitudinal data from Add Health
on more than 15,000 young people who were in middle or high
school in the mid 1990s. Unfortunately, because the
questions about family structure in the Add Health survey
were not very detailed, respondents had to be divided into
two simple categories: those whose biological father had
been absent since birth, and those who, as of their
adolescence, had lived with their biological father at some
point (whether for a short period or their entire
childhood). The data on educational attainment
measuring whether respondents had attained at least some
college education, whether or not they had completed a
degree came from a later wave of the survey, when
respondents were 24 to 32 years old.
Boys get in more trouble than
girls from elementary school onward, and the gender
difference is greater among children from fatherless
homes.
The results showed that among young
people who had lived with their fathers at some point, 72.1
percent of women and 63.1 percent of men had some college
education, meaning men enrolled at 87.5 percent the rate of
women. Among those with no father present, the
figures were 61.3 percent for women and 49.2 percent for
men; the men enrolled at 80.3 percent the rate of women. In
other words, a gender gap favoring women was evident in both
groups, but the gap was larger among young people whose
father was absent. The ratio of the relative risks (0.918)
indicated that the finding was statistically significant:
Males were disproportionately less likely than females
to attend college if they came from a family in which the
father had been absent from birth. The method Doherty
et al. used to analyze the Add Health data did not allow for
controlling for the usual background characteristics;
however, the relative education levels of the Add Health
samples male-female sibling pairs, who would share
many socio-demographic traits, were consistent with those of
the overall sample.
The study does not go into much detail
on exactly why father absence appears to undermine
boys academic achievement more than that of girls. In
Wayward
Sons: The Emerging Gender Gap in Labor Markets and
Education, a 2013 report
from Third Way, David Autor and Melanie Wasserman provide
some answers. First, long before gender and family-structure
differences in college graduation appear, there emerge
differences in academic performance and behavior at school.
Boys get in more trouble than girls from elementary
school onward, and the gender difference is greater among
children from fatherless homes.
That may be due, Autor and Wasserman
write, to parents usage of time. More-highly-educated
parents have increased the amount of time they devote to
child-care activities in the last 20 years. Less-educated
parents who are more likely to be low-income and
single have done so, too, but not to the same extent.
If boys are more responsive to parental inputs (or the
absence thereof) than are girls, Autor and Wasserman
summarize, then it is possible that the gender
gradient in behavioral and academic development could be
magnified in single-parent households.
And then, of course, there is role
modeling: If children aim to emulate the adult roles
of their same-sex parent, then girls may increasingly expect
to fully support both themselves and their children whereas,
conversely, males may come to anticipate a less central or
more transient role. Doherty, Willoughby, and Wilde
make a similar point:
As more boys grow up without
their father in the home, and as women (especially in
low-income and working-class communities) are viewed as
the more stable achievers, boys and girls alike
[may] come to see males as having a lower
achievement orientation and less aptitude for higher
education. In the context of persistent influences of
family structure, public policy, and macroeconomic
forces, at some point social norms regarding academic
goals become self-reinforcing within peer groups and
perhaps even with parents. Stated simply, college becomes
something that many girls, but only some boys, do
the opposite of the earlier cultural norm.
Sure, plenty of men (and women) who
dont go to college still find decent jobs and lead
happy lives. But in light of the many benefits associated
with college degrees from higher income and a lower
unemployment rate to greater marital stability and better
health we shouldnt rest easy when any group
finds them increasingly difficult to attain.
Source: www.nationalreview.com/article/425506/gender-gap-college-fatherless-households
The Cost of Gender
Inequiality
Throughout the world women often receive less education and
are not employed at the same rate as their male counter
parts. In the United States, there are nearly twice as many
men as women with professional or doctoral degrees, and 70.5
percent of men either have a job or are looking for one
compared to just 58.1 percent of women. And in countries
like Yemen, gender disparities are seen even in secondary
school where boys enroll at a rate 20 percentage points
higher than girls.
There are many potential variables
that could account for this inequality societal
norms, gender discrimination, or the challenges surrounding
childcare but what is becoming more obvious is that
this gap is negatively impacting entire populations. Beyond
issues of gender equity and human development, research
points to damaging economic consequences for regions with
large gender gaps in education and employment.
In their empirical analysis of regions
around the world between 1960 and 2000, authors Stephan
Klasen and Francesca Lamanna expound upon the growing body
of research indicating that gender gaps in education and
employment have negative economic consequences. Overall, the
authors find that reducing gender gaps is correlated to
positive regional economic growth.
The authors compare economic growth
rates to education and employment gaps for several regions
including the Middle East and North Africa, Latin America
and the Caribbean, East Asia and the Pacific, Organization
for Economic Co-operation and Development, South Asia,
Sub-Saharan Africa, and Eastern Europe and Central Asia.
They confirmed that gender gaps in education and employment
create statistically significant regional economic
inefficiencies. They also provide insight into which regions
have been hurt the most by their respective gender gaps and
why this may be the case.
By excluding women
from various forms of higher education, regions reduce
their productivity.
The authors outline several
explanations for why a gender gap in education and
employment could lower economic development. Previous
studies suggest that two primary reasons the education gap
is harmful to the economy are inefficient uses of human
capital and increased fertility rate. By excluding women
from various forms of higher education, regions reduce their
productivity. Women that could be valuable assets to the
economy are not achieving their full capabilities in the
labor market, and the economy suffers because its human
capital is limited and competition is reduced.
Arguably more vital to economic
success is the population shift due to fertility rates.
Previous research shows that low levels of education are
highly correlated with higher pregnancy rates. This leads to
populations with a labor force that cannot support the rest
of the population. By contrast, regions with a low education
gender gap have shown what Bloom and Williamson (1998) call
a demographic gift, meaning there will be more
active than inactive individuals in the labor force, thus
boosting economic productivity.
Klasen and Lamanna also note that
previous research indicates that a high employment gender
gap reduces the talent pool for prospective employers. This
underutilization of human capital and labor has a negative
impact on overall economic efficiency. Research suggests
this is especially true of manufacturing export-based
economies that depend on a high degree of labor. There is
also evidence to suggest that women with the ability to make
financial decisions tend to save more, are less prone to
corruption, and tend to spend money on investments like
healthcare and education, which leads to positive economic
growth.
The study by Klasen and Lamanna
reveals that South Asia (SA), the Middle East and North
Africa (MENA), and Sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) have large
economic and education gender gaps correlating with weak
economic growth. MENA and SA are reducing their education
gaps and could see a demographic gift soon, but the
countries have suffered more than regions near them, likes
East Asia, that have done a better job reducing their gender
gaps. In addition, the MENA region has reduced its education
gap faster than SA, leading to more economic growth. In SA,
only Bangladesh worked to reduce its gender gaps and has
experienced an economic boom similar to East
Asia.
Persistent gender
gaps not only impact women, but are damaging to the
socioeconomic development of entire
populations.
Given the current data, it would be
advisable for countries to enact policies that reduce their
economic and education gender gaps. Persistent gender gaps
not only impact women, but are damaging to the socioeconomic
development of entire populations. From this perspective,
gender inequality is a disadvantage to societies that must
compete in global markets where countries around the world
are taking steps to decrease gender gaps and improving their
economies.
Article Source: The Impact of
Gender Inequality in Education and Employment on Economic
Growth: New Evidence for a Panel of Countries, Stephan
Klasen and Francesca Lamanna, Feminist Economics, 2009.
Source: chicagopolicyreview.org/2014/10/22/the-cost-of-gender-inequality/
Teachers
unaware of growing gender gaps in classrooms
A gap in reading and math scores still exists in lower
grades, with boys continuing to outpace girls in math, and
girls ahead of boys in reading, two University of Illinois
education professors say.
Using national longitudinal data to
perform their analysis, Joseph P. Robinson and Sarah
Lubienski investigated male and female achievement in math
and reading, looking for when gender gaps first appeared and
where in the distribution the gaps were most
prevalent.
Except for kindergarteners in the 99th
percentile, boys and girls generally start out on equal
footing in math competency. In elementary school, girls
throughout the distribution lose ground to boys in math
achievement before eventually regaining some ground in
middle school, according to research published by the
professors in the American Educational Research
Journal.
"If you just look at the average gap,
there is no gap in math between boys and girls when they
start kindergarten," Robinson said. "But when you start to
break it down throughout the distribution, taking a look at
the low- and high-achieving girls and boys, that's where we
see that there's a gap favoring boys at the upper-most
extreme of the distribution. The 99th percentile of boys is
outscoring the 99th percentile of girls."
Over time, as students progress
through elementary school, the gap "begins to widen,
favoring boys in the lower part of the distribution,"
Robinson said. "By third grade, you can see it throughout
the whole range of kids."
Robinson and Lubienski also compared
teachers' assessments of boys and girls. They discovered
that teachers seem to overestimate girls' mathematics
achievement relative to boys, rating girls higher than boys
in both subjects, even when cognitive assessments suggest
that boys have a math advantage.
"Our results suggest that there is
still a gender gap, not only with achievement, but with
teachers' perceptions," Lubienski said.
Based in part on other research, the
professors suspect that teachers might be mistaking girls'
compliance in the classroom for comprehension, a topic that
the researchers are exploring in a forthcoming
study.
"We thought that teachers might rate
boys higher in math, but we found that even when boys are
outscoring girls, the teachers think the girls are
outscoring the boys," Lubienski said. "This might be because
girls tend to be perceived as 'good girls' in the classroom,
and then teachers assume that they understand the material
because they complete their work and don't cause
trouble."
The researchers say that there's also
a gap in reading that favors girls. Although the gap
favoring girls generally narrows over time, it also
eventually widens among low-achieving girls and boys, who
struggle to keep up with their classmates.
"Clearly, the boys start out behind
the girls in reading achievement," Lubienski said. "In
general, the mid-achieving boys eventually catch up, but the
lowest-achieving boys don't. In other words, if you're a boy
and you're really struggling to read, you most likely won't
catch up with your peers. It's those boys at the bottom that
teachers should be most concerned about when it comes to
reading."
The issue of gender gaps in math and
reading in U. S. schools has been an ongoing one in
education circles, with some researchers arguing that a
gender gap doesn't exist in math anymore, something that was
concluded from looking at test results from several states.
"There have been debates about whether there really is a
gender gap in math," Lubienski said.
"But our research looked at national
data, and they show that there is indeed still a gender gap
in math. It's small, but it's there, and it grows between
kindergarten and fifth grade."
As a country, the U.S. seems to have
more of a gender gap in early elementary education than in
most countries, the researchers say. One hypothesis to
explain the gap could be that the U.S. has first and second
grade female teachers who are "math-anxious."
"I've seen a surprising number of
teachers who want to teach in the lower grades because
they're scared of math," Lubienski said. "Other research has
shown a link between math-anxious teachers and girls' math
performance, so that could also account for the early gender
disparities that we found."
Instead of having one teacher for all
of the subjects, Robinson and Lubienski believe that having
math specialists teaching in the elementary grades, and not
just generalists who teach every subject, could help to
close the achievement gap.
"If you have a teacher who actually
likes math, rather than one who just wants to get it over
with, then I think it would be helpful, especially
considering that we have these early gaps and other
countries don't," Lubienski said. "There's some debate about
whether kids need to stay with one teacher because it
nurtures them. But from a math education standpoint, having
dedicated math specialists is certainly worth
exploring."
For education policymakers, the
professors say their research suggests that teachers need to
intervene earlier when students struggle.
"We should target effective
interventions for the content domains where we see gaps, and
we must ensure that these interventions are in place by the
grades in which we start to see gaps emerge, which our
research suggests is earlier than previously thought,"
Robinson said.
"We can't just ignore the gender gap
and think that it's done," Lubienski said. "There's been
some concern about boys being short-changed in school, and
our research supports that claim for boys who have
difficulty with reading."
"But teachers might also underestimate
the attention that young girls need in math," Lubienski
said. "So we need to pay attention not only to the
low-achieving boys who are struggling with reading, but also
to the girls both the high-achievers as well as the
low-achievers as they learn math in the early
grades."
Source: phys.org/news/2011-03-teachers-unaware-gender-gaps-classrooms.html
The
Link Between Sports and Academic
Performance
Whether children, teenagers, or
adults studies have consistently demonstrated that
physically active people remain healthier and are able to
perform better on tests of cerebral or intellectual ability.
Some studies even indicate that the results are sharp and
immediate even a quick 5-minute walk can yield
immediate results.
Most studies show that the more
exercise one gets, the higher ones mental faculties
and cerebral performance. Yet, the picture is somewhat more
complicated when it comes to college students who are also
serious athletes. When these high-level athletes have to
stay in shape, attend practices, travel to meets or games
away from home, and still fulfill all the requirements of
other college students, things can get tricky, and the
measure of academic performance is no longer just a grade on
a single exam.
While some college athletes experience
difficulty balancing the responsibilities of their sport
with the responsibilities of their academics, many student
athletes actually find that the high degree of organization
required to accomplish both leads them to be highly
successful in both areas.
In general, it has been scientifically
demonstrated time and again that physical exercise is
tightly correlated with mental acuity. A 2010 article in the
Washington Post cited John J. Ratey, a Harvard University
psychiatrist who synthesized volumes of research for his
intriguing 2008 book Spark: The Revolutionary New Science of
Exercise and the Brain. In his book, Ratey describes taking
MRI scans of the brains of sedentary people who have
suddenly improved their fitness and increased volume
in the hippocampus and frontal and temporal lobes, the
regions of the brain associated with cognitive functioning.
The hippocampus in particular is associated with memory and
learning. (1)
Moreover, a recent article of the BBC
(British Broadcasting Corporation) cited a university study
carried out on about 5,000 children and adolescents, which
found links between exercise and exam success in English,
mathematics and science and discovered an increase in
performance for every extra 17 minutes boys exercised, and
12 minutes for girls (2). The study was carried out by the
universities of Strathclyde and Dundee, and found physical
activity particularly beneficial to girls performance
at science; the authors said this could be a chance finding
or reflect gender differences in the impact of physical
activity on the brain. Overall, though, children who carried
out regular exercise, not only did better academically at 11
but also at 13 and in their exams at 16, the study
suggested. Dr Josie Booth of Dundee University in the UK,
one of the leaders of the British study, said:
Physical activity is more than just important for your
physical health. There are other benefits and that is
something that should be especially important to parents,
policy-makers and people involved in education.
(Ibid)
In addition, a 2010 report published
by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) of
the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services stated that
across 50 studies undertaken on the subject of physical
activity and academic performance, as reported in 43
separate academic articles, there were a total of 251
associations between physical activity and academic
performance, which represented measures of academic
achievement, academic behavior, and cognitive skills and
attitudes (3). In the studies examined by the CDC report,
increased time in physical education appears to have a
positive relationship or no relationship with academic
achievement. Increased time in physical education does not
appear to have a negative relationship with academic
achievement. Eleven of the 14 studies found one or more
positive associations between school-based physical
education and indicators of academic performance; the
remaining three studies found no significant
associations. (Ibid.) It is important to note that
most of the scientific literature on the link between sports
or physical exercise and performance in specifically
academic settings are in reference to children and
adolescents. However, for people of all ages, the overall
connection between keeping the body in shape and the brain
in tip-top shape cannot be denied.
A Complex Picture: Elite-Level
Athletes in College Sports
While universities across the country
offer a large number of collegiate sports for students, only
a handful get wide recognition. Often those big-business
sports mostly football and basketball feature
students who sometimes having difficulty making the academic
cut, for various reasons. For this reason, sometimes sports
have gotten a bad rap as a negative factor in college
academic performance. Yet this may be an unrelated issue
some students mediocre grades may simply
reflect those students sharper focus on excelling in
sports than in academics which is not surprising in
sports that offer the possibility of professional
recruitment post-college.
The Global Post remarks that although
student athletes performance can vary by sport, with
the athletes in the most competitive and popular sports
tending to exhibit lower academic performance, gender also
plays a role (4). Female athletes consistently outperform
both male athletes and male non-athletes, says the piece,
citing an article in The New York Times. Worth noting too is
that even women recruited specifically for their athletic
prowess earn high marks, with average GPAs just .06 points
behind female non-athletes, suggesting that the mere fact of
participating in elite-level college athletics may bear
little relationship to academic success.
Yet grades and GPA averages are not
always the only measure of academic success. Many student
athletes work hard to find a balance between their
responsibilities. While some students may not have personal
responsibilities, athletics, or the need to earn a living
outside their studies, and post straight As, other
students may have any or all of these other responsibilities
and yet manage to post 3.9 GPAs throughout college. With all
those responsibilities outside the classroom, no one could
deny that 3.9 to be an impressive achievement. In short,
while there are no comprehensive data that compare student
athletes grades to those of their non-athlete peers,
it is clear that the difference really comes down to
personal drive, determination, and ability to organize and
balance.
Getting Some Exercise Means Getting
More Done
Ultimately, countless health benefits
are brought on by physical activity be it devotion to
practicing an individual sport, team sports pick-up games,
the weekend trip to the gym, or simply a daily walk around
the block. When we take care of our bodies, our minds follow
the positive pattern, and we are able to be the best we can
be at academics and beyond.
Footnotes
Bernstein, Lenny. A
growing body of evidence links exercise and mental
acuity, published May 25, 2010 at
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2010/05/24/AR2010052402608.html
. Access date: February 26, 2013.
Exercise boosts
academic performance of teenagers, published
October 21, 2013 at
http://www.bbc.com/news/uk-scotland-24608813 . Access date:
February 26, 2013.
The Association
Between School-Based Physical Activity, Including Physical
Education, and Academic Performance. July 2010,
available at
http://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/health_and_academics/pdf/pa-pe_paper.pdf.
Access date: February 26, 2013.
Academics of College
Athletes vs. Non-Athletes. Global Post. Available at
http://everydaylife.globalpost.com/academics-college-athletes-vs-nonathletes-16678.html
. Access date: February 26, 2013.
Source: www.fnu.edu/the-link-between-sports-and-academic-performance/
A Degree Goal:
To Close a Gender Gap That Favors Women
When Victor Sáenz, an education professor at the
University of Texas at Austin, began to focus his research
nearly a decade ago on the plight of men in education, he
experienced some pushback, even from fellow
academics.
Early on, Id get a lot of
questions, said Mr. Sáenz, who in 2010 started
a mentoring group for male Hispanic students called Project
Males Mentoring to Achieve Latino Educational
Success. I wouldnt say criticisms, but certainly
apprehension or resistance to focusing on this
issue.
But as degree attainment among men has
continued to lag that of women, more state policy makers are
looking at the issue in a bid to prevent the gap from
significantly affecting the state.
National interest is also rising. Last
month, Mr. Sáenz helped lead an online seminar hosted
by the White House, which has started initiatives on the
education of Hispanic and black men, the populations with
the lowest rates of higher education success.
If half the population is
systematically lagging behind the other half, thats
going to be a real drag on our ability to meet our goals and
secure any kind of prosperity for our future, said Mr.
Sáenz, adding that the increased attention,
particularly in a state with rapidly changing demographics,
is probably driven by economic imperatives. (The University
of Texas at Austin is a corporate sponsor of The Texas
Tribune.)
David Gardner, the Texas Higher
Education Coordinating Boards deputy commissioner for
academic planning and policy, said he expected the issue to
be a plank in the states next long-term higher
education plan. Narrowing the gender gap was not an explicit
goal in the states previous 15-year plan, which
expires next year.
When it comes to getting their
degrees, female students outperform their male peers in
every ethnic and socioeconomic group. But attainment is
particularly low among Hispanic and black men.
Of the Texas students who enrolled in
eighth grade in 2001, about 23.3 percent of the female
students had earned some sort of higher education
credential, compared with 15.7 percent of the male students.
Fewer than 10 percent of the black and Hispanic male
students had earned a degree or certificate.
A report released this year by the
Austin-based Center for Community College Student Engagement
said black male students reported the highest level of
participation in educational and support programs at
community colleges. But when compared with their white and
Hispanic peers, they had the lowest success
rates.
Kay McClenney, the centers
former director, said this conundrum might
result, in part, from well-intentioned but ineffective
support services.
Way too often, she said,
the programs are designed to fix the student rather
than designed to fix the institution so that it is more
effective at serving the student.
Richard Rhodes, the president of
Austin Community College, agreed. At times, not
intentionally, we think were doing the right thing,
but we create barriers to opening up pathways for
students. (Richard Rhodes is a donor to The
Tribune.)
Austin Community College is a member
of the Texas Education Consortium for Male Students of
Color, which Mr. Sáenz and his colleagues created
last year. The group hopes to develop more effective
strategies for boosting student achievement among minority
men.
Right now, there are a lot of
homegrown or boutique programs, Mr. Sáenz said.
But there has not been a very good culture of
evidence.
Source: www.nytimes.com/2014/05/11/us/a-degree-goal-to-close-a-gender-gap-that-favors-women.html?_r=0
Does
Gender Diversiity Improeve Performance
Over the past six years, companies with at least some female
board representation outperformed those with no women on the
board in terms of share price performance, according to the
latest study by the Credit Suisse Research
Institute.
In the latest Credit Suisse Research
Institute study, the issue of gender diversity and
performance is considered from a global perspective. The
study analyses the performance of close to 2,400 companies
with and without female board members from 2005
onwards.
Women on the Board Contribute to
Outperformance
The key finding is that, in a
like-for-like comparison, companies with at least one woman
on the board would have outperformed stocks with no women on
the board by 26 percent over the course of the last 6 years.
However, there is a clear split between relative performance
over 2005 to 2007 and the post 2008 performance. In the
middle of the decade when economic growth was relatively
robust, there was little difference in share price
performance between companies with or without women on the
board. Almost all of the outperformance in the back-test has
been delivered post 2008, since the macro environment
deteriorated and volatility increased. In other words,
stocks with greater gender diversity on their boards
generally look defensive: they tend to perform best when
markets are falling, deliver higher average ROEs through the
cycle, exhibit less volatility in earnings and typically
have lower gearing ratios.
The bottom line is that relative
outperformance of stocks with women on the board looks
unlikely to be entirely consistent, but the evidence
suggests that a bit more balance on the board brings with it
a bit less volatility and a bit more balance through the
cycle.
Why Do Women on the Board Enhance
Performance?
The report identifies seven key
reasons why greater gender diversity could be correlated
with stronger corporate performance:
(1) A signal of a Better
Company
There is a body of research that
suggests that appointment of women to the board is a sign
that the company is already doing well rather than a signal
of greater things to come. The Research Institute's analysis
found that it was indeed the larger companies that, to some
extent by definition, have already performed well, that were
more likely to have women on the board representatives.
However, the strong outperformance of companies with women
on the board, even in an exclusive comparison of the large
caps, suggests there may be other facets to the
relationship.
(2) Greater Effort Across the
Board
Evidence suggests that greater team
diversity (including gender diversity) can lead to better
average performance. Research conducted by Professor
Katherine Philips at Columbia University has shown that
majority groups improve their own performance in response to
minority involvement producing better average outcomes in
more diverse environments.
(3) A Better Mix of Leadership
Skills
McKinsey and NASA have conducted
various studies on leadership skills and have shown that
women are particularly good at defining responsibilities
clearly as well as being strong on mentoring and coaching
employees. Hence, the idea that a degree of gender diversity
at the board level would foster a better balance in
leadership skills within the company may hold
merit.
(4) Access to a Wider Talent
Pool
Data from UNESCO shows that by 2010,
the proportion of female graduates across the world came to
a median average of 54 percent. This compares to a median
average of 51 percent female graduates in 2000. The trend
towards an even greater proportion of female graduates looks
set to continue if female success at primary and secondary
school level is any guide. Hence, any company that achieves
greater gender diversity is more likely to be able to tap
into the widest possible pool of talent that is implicit in
these graduation statistics.
(5) A Better Reflection of the
Consumer Decision Maker
To the extent that women are
responsible for household spending decisions, it makes sense
that a corporate board with female representation may
enhance the understanding of customer preferences. Not
surprisingly, consumer-facing industries already rank among
those with the greater proportion of women on the
board.
(6) Improved Corporate
Governance
There is unusually strong consensus
within the academic research that a greater number of women
on the board improves performance on corporate and social
governance metrics.
The Research Institute's analysis of
the MSCI AC World constituents showed that stocks with women
on board are more likely to have lower levels of gearing
than their peer group where there are no women on the board.
Lower relative debt levels have been a useful determinant of
equity market outperformance over the last four years,
delivering average outperformance of 2.5 percent per annum
over the last 20 years and 6.5 percent per annum over the
last four years.
Gender Diversity and
Senior Management
Gender diversity within the senior
management team has become an increasingly topical issue
for three reasons:
- Although the proportion of
women at board level generally remains very low, it is
changing. On our numbers, only 41 percent of MSCI ACWI
stocks had any women on the board at the end of 2005,
but this had increased to 59 percent by the end of
2011.
- Government intervention in this
area has increased. In the last 5 years, 7 countries
have passed legislation mandating female board
representation and 8 countries have set non-mandatory
targets. China may even add a woman to the powerful
nine "man" Standing Committee of the Politburo towards
the end of 2012.
- Most interestingly, the debate
around the topic has shifted from an issue of fairness
and equality to a question of superior performance. If
gender diversity on the board implies a greater
probability of corporate success then it would make
sense to pursue such an objective regardless of any
government directive.
Source: www.credit-suisse.com/us/en/about-us/research/research-institute/news-and-videos/articles/news-and-expertise/2012/07/en/does-gender-diversity-improve-performance.html
Culture,
Socialisation And The Gender Education Gap
Last but not least I will discuss the role of culture and
the socialisation of boys in the gender education gap and in
producing boys lower academic performance. As has been
alluded to in the previous articles, boys and girls are
raised and socialised differently in our society. This has
important implications for institutions like schools,
because it means that young boys and girls will behave
differently and have different skill sets when they start
their education. So we cannot expect to treat them or
educate them the same way. Of course there are also inherent
psychological differences between the genders, but many of
these differences are small and/or in areas that fail to
explain the scholastic achievement gap between boys and
girls. We cannot expect to raise boys to be square pegs
fitting into the round hole of our feminised education
system and be successful.
Studies have shown that from birth,
boys and girls are treated differently by their parents.
Often baby and toddler girls will be spoken to and read to
more often. In addition when they cry, parents are quicker
to rush to their aid. It is not surprising then that girls
often have wider vocabularies and reading ability than boys
when they start school. It is not unexpected either that
boys are less likely to ask for help when they need
assistance at school. What the gender education gap tells us
is that if anything, we should be speaking and reading to
boys much more often. We should also be encouraging boys to
seek help when they are in trouble (although both genders
should be raised to have some degree of independence and
resilience). Perhaps if we raised them this way, the boys
would be better suited to our school systems and a
curriculum that is highly language based. If boys sort
assistance, rather than gave up and switched off, they would
also be less likely to fall behind in class.
Perhaps the most damaging aspect of
social influence impacting boys performance in
education is our Western culture. I remember when I was
growing up being constantly bombarded with messages on TV
implying that boys and men were stupid. One example that
comes to mind is the portrayal of men in the Simpsons. Bart
and Homer are portrayed as idiots and Lisa and Marge are
portrayed as smart. When we do see a male character in the
show come across as smart, like Martin Prince or Frank
Grimes, they are portrayed as geeks or losers. We see this
treatment again in shows like TheBig Bang Theory. On the
rare occasion now when a male is smart on our TV shows, he
must be some weird, unpopular nerd.
Again we see in our culture the
promotion of many male role models in sport, but basically
no attempt to showcase smart, academic or innovative men
(Steve Jobs fortunately was one, but he does not have much
company). In the movies the guy everyone likes and who gets
the ladies, is often not that smart, but instead relies on
his physical strength and aggression. Frequently he is a
criminal, violent or has questionable character and personal
history. There is a lack of a strong cultural masculine
identity in modern Western culture that is associated with
intellectual pursuit and other characteristics that are the
key to being successful in the modern world (as opposed to
the old warrior archetype). Consequently, many boys lack an
up-to-date cultural reference of masculinity to direct their
life pursuits in ways that lead to success in modern
society. They want to be the next wrestler or basketballer,
rather than the next nobel prize winner or CEO. This is
partially why boys tune out in class, as they are not shown
the link between academic performance and future success in
the modern world by cultural references to
masculinity.
So what takes the place of a modern
healthy male identity in our Western culture? Well in
newspapers, in advertisements and the TV news, there is no
end to the amount of misandric garbage telling young boys
that men are lazy and stupid. We have people like Hanna
Rosin, Maureen Dowd and so forth, peddling the same old
tired story that men are redundant, obsolete and no good. Do
not get me started on the ads. Prof. Paul Nathanson and
Prof. Katherine Young have analysed the negative portrayal
of men in the media, TV shows, ads, radio, books and popular
culture and have written a book about it. Their findings
highlight a truly pervasive social phenomenon, particularly
in Western society, that promotes and encourages misandry.
Examples of misandry are not exceptional, they are now a
normal part of our culture and are found throughout our
media, newsprint, TV, radio, online content and pop culture.
The messages that get sent to boys from all these different
cultural channels of our society, is that we expect that
they be stupid and that it is not only okay, but funny and
encouraged. If you are a smart boy, then you are a geek and
undesirable to other men and to women.
Remember that boys are still forming
their identities when they are exposed to these harmful
messages. They are quite impressionable and lack the
critical thinking of adults to counteract these messages. To
make matters worse, the lack of fathers and male role models
at school, further increases the influence such messages
have on the socialisation and psychological development of
boys. Is it any surprise then, that young boys glorify
sports, violence, reckless behaviour, aggression and
stupidity over intelligence and academia? Absolutely not. Of
those traits only one or two are positive, sports and
aggression in the right contexts. However, something like
study or being smart is discouraged in boys and
men.
The impact of misandry in our culture
on young boys thinking, becomes amplified many fold when
they begin to play and interact with their peers. These
twisted social norms they learn from the media, TV and our
culture etc, are reinforced and policed in boys social
groups. The peer group pressure on boys to conform to social
norms of disobedience, reckless behaviour, violence and
being good at sport but bad academically (or not being a
"nerd") is intense. The jock gets the girls and is popular
and the nerd is a loner. Honestly after having gone through
primary school and later on to an all boys high school, I
confirm this is indeed the mentality. I was very independent
compared to most of the other boys and really did not care
what they thought of me. I worked and studied hard and
achieved really good grades. I can tell you though that the
pressure on me to stop being such a nerd was
intense. Getting an A+ or being awarded Dux put a target on
your head. If you did well at sport though, suddenly
everyone liked you.
What was interesting to note is that
this type of treatment did not happen anywhere near as often
with the girl groups at primary school or at our sister all
girls high school (when I frequently spoke with them or saw
them on the train home or outside school socially). They
would praise each other if they did well at school and
encouraged each other in their study. Instead of talking
about sports, they would be talking about their school
subjects or which university or career they were going to
pursue (not all the time but definitely more frequently than
the boys). Very rarely did they call each other nerds and if
they did it was often in reference to a smart yet
unattractive or unpopular girl. She was called a nerd for
reasons other than being smart.
Another thing I noticed at school, was
the stark difference in academic performance when comparing
my Caucasian male peers, to my Indian, Jewish and Asian
counterparts. Boys from these ethnic backgrounds performed
far better academically than the white Anglo-Saxon males.
Why? Because in those cultures education was still highly
valued in men. It was the Indian, Jewish and Asian boys
getting all the academic awards and scholarships amongst the
boys. They were performing as well as or better than the
bulk of the female students. At home the ethos of valuing
education is instilled in these boys from a young age. The
male parent is often around and fatherlessness is almost
unheard of. Again I dont make these remarks to be
racist but instead to compliment these cultures.
Frankly we could learn a thing or two
from our Eastern counterparts when it comes to the education
of boys. We need to more strictly regulate the portrayal of
men on TV and in film. We also need more positive and
intelligent (of particular importance) male role models in
these mediums. To achieve these goals, there needs to be
more activism to keep the corporate media and entertainment
industry in line, because they have enormous influence on
our culture. This especially applies in present times, where
many boys are watching several hours of TV a day and being
exposed to vast amounts of online content. We need to be
spending less time promoting male athletes and more time
promoting our brilliant male thinkers (like Steve Jobs, not
that I own anything Apple, but I respected the man). We need
to eliminate the misandric articles and reports in the news
that are either baseless or built upon poor quality research
or a misunderstanding of it.
Until that happens and we change how
we define masculinity in our culture, I expect we will keep
reinforcing all the behaviours that have led to and
perpetuated the gender education gap. If these negative
cultural messages and bad stereotypes of masculinity keep
telling boys we expect them to perform badly academically,
the pygmalion effect tells us it will become a self
fulfilling prophecy and this will be reinforced in their
peer groups (where they will learn and internalise a twisted
form of what masculinity is in their pscyhe). In short, the
declining academic performance of boys is a product of our
modern man-bashing culture and the imprint it leaves on
their identity from a young age.
Again
I urge people to look at the study on the classroom
environment, which was a classic example of the pygmalion
effect and demonstrated it's negative impact on boys
academic performance. I
discussed it at length in my article on the classroom
environment and the gender education gap.
Boys are failing at school because the
classroom environment and curriculum is hostile, irrelevant
and unaccommodating to their masculinity. They are failing
because they have no male role models as frames of reference
and guidance at school or at home. They are failing because
they are not being raised with the life skills necessary to
make the most of their education. They are failing because
of the negative influence of feminism in our education
systems and the bias that induces in the educational
environment and cirriculum. They are failing because our
culture and society, create, spread and promote messages
that attack and downplay male intellect and academic
achievement. They are failing because our culture and
society spread and endorse misandric messages that tell them
that they are no good because they have a penis. They are
failing because their peers exert pressure on them to
conform to these twisted social norms and negative
associations with masculinity. They are failing because our
culture fails to provide boys with proper cultural
references of masculintiy and what it takes to be successful
as a man in the modern age.
Consequently for all these reasons and
more, boys are not motivated to learn or do well at school
and they predictably put in no effort, switch off and play
up in class. Boys will be boys right? A false misandric
justification that leads to a self fulfilling prophecy. The
scientific evidence says boys have it in them to perform
just as well as girls, if they are taught, socialised and
encouraged to apply themselves just as girls are. How sexist
indeed is a comment like boys will be boys! Did we make that
excuse 40 years ago when we tried to help women in
education? No!
Source: hubpages.com/education/Culture-And-Socialisation-And-The-Gender-Education-Gap
Gender Differences in the Classroom:
Physical, Cognitive & Behavioral
Growing up, did you ever observe gender differences among
girls and boys in school? Do you still observe gender
differences as an adult? There are established gender
differences noted in a variety of contexts. This lesson will
explore specific differences in physical and motor skills,
cognitive abilities and more.
Gender Differences in the
Classroom
Researchers have identified several
areas of difference between boys and girls. While some of
these differences may be perpetuated by stereotypes, all are
real and observable.
Boys tend to be more physically active
than girls and often have trouble with sedentary
activities.
Physical Activity and Motor
Skills
Within the realm of physical activity
and motor skills, researchers have found that boys are
generally more active than girls. Boys tend to have trouble
sitting still for lengthy periods, and therefore do not
enjoy activities that are sedentary in nature. Reading,
coloring, and activities that require sitting still are more
difficult for boys.
Pre-puberty boys and girls have
similar potential for physical and motor growth, although
girls have a slight edge in fine motor skills. After
puberty, boys have a biological advantage in physical
activity due to their height and muscular development. Boys
tend to develop their physical and motor skills more through
participation in organized sports.
It's important to understand the
educational implications of gender differences between boys
and girls. Curriculum, especially involving physical
education classes and group sports, should provide equal
opportunities for boys and girls to maximize their physical
well-being and athletic skills.
Cognitive Abilities and Achievement
Motivation
When taking standardized assessments,
boys and girls typically perform the same. This is due in
part to testing standards in validity and reliability.
Researchers have identified gender differences in cognitive
abilities, however.
Girls have been found to perform
slightly higher in verbal ability exercises, while boys tend
to perform slightly higher in visual-spatial exercises. It
is important to keep in mind that these differences are
relatively small.
Boys do tend to show greater
variability in cognitive abilities. Boys, more so than
girls, appear at the extreme upper and lower ends of the
assessment spectrum.
Girls tend to consistently earn higher
grades in school and are, on average, more concerned about
doing well in school. They are typically more engaged in
classroom activities, persist, and are more likely to
graduate.
Girls tend to gravitate toward
activities and courses that they know they will do well in.
By the time students are in high school, the courses they
select reveal distinct gender differences. Boys typically
enroll in math and physical science classes, while girls
typically choose language and literature-based
courses.
In terms of educational importance,
curriculum should involve opportunities for boys and girls
to explore areas that they may not feel high self-efficacy
towards (such as reading and writing for boys and science
and math for girls). These opportunities should promote
achievement and appreciation for the unfamiliar or
uncomfortable subject matter.
Interpersonal Behavior and
Relationships
Researchers have identified gender
differences in the way boys and girls interact with their
peers. Boys are typically more physically aggressive than
girls, especially in elementary and middle school years.
Boys are more likely to engage in aggressive and bullying
behaviors without being provoked.
Girls can be equally aggressive, but
they demonstrate their aggressiveness in nonphysical ways.
Spreading rumors, giving mean stares and alienating girls
from other friends are examples of these
behaviors.
Boys tend to hang out in large groups
of other boys. Their activities usually involve physical
play, group games and risk-taking. They enjoy competition as
well. Girls engage in more cooperative play and are more
aware of other girls' mental and emotional
states.
The
educational importance of being aware of interpersonal
behavior and relationship differences is that classrooms
should provide numerous opportunities for cooperative group
work and frequent interaction with classmates in order to
take advantage of boys' natural tendency to play in big
groups and girls' natural tendencies to engage in
cooperative activities.
Sense of Self and
Self-Esteem
When talking about sense of self and
self-esteem, boys typically hold a higher overall sense of
self-worth than girls beginning in upper elementary or
middle school. This could be due in part to boys' tendencies
to overestimate their abilities and girls' tendencies to
underestimate. Boys have higher self-confidence and view
themselves as being better athletes and problem
solvers.
Beginning at puberty, girls tend to
hold a lower sense of self-worth and rate their physical
appearance less favorably than boys.
Both boys and girls rate themselves
higher in academic areas that are stereotypical for their
gender, such as math for boys and literature for
girls.
Classroom Behavior
When talking about classroom behavior
and gender differences, researchers have identified
differences in the classroom, with boys tending to be more
active participants by talking more and asking questions.
Boys also tend to dominate group discussions and ignore
girls' ideas and requests. Girls tend to be less likely to
publicly volunteer or ask questions, possibly for fear of
looking incompetent in front of their peers.
In terms of educational importance,
educators may sometimes want to group girls with other girls
and boys with other boys to ensure girls actively
participate in classroom activitie
To unlock this lesson you must be a
Study.com Member. Create your account
Source: study.com/academy/lesson/gender-differences-in-the-classroom-physical-cognitive-behavioral.html
Gender proves
large factor in academic performance
Dont be too excited, its an estrogen
fest, junior Kristin Miller recalled telling a friend
from N.C. State University, who had come to Elon for a
visit. Indeed, the imbalance between genders at Elon simply
cannot be overlooked. The seemingly problematic girl to guy
ratio is not just a local problem, though. Elon is an
example of the national trend, in which university males are
slowly becoming a minority group.
A recent article in The Seattle Times
outlined the national trend. Female college graduates
fewer than half of all graduates a decade ago
now outnumber their male counterparts in most industrialized
countries, the article stated. The current number of
female graduates is amazing compared to ten years ago. A
recent CBS newscast stated that campuses are now around 60
percent female, with women earning 170,000 more bachelor
degrees each year then men. Women are streaming into
business schools and medical schools, and will be the
majority at the nations law schools.
This trend is not only due to the fact
that more women than men are applying to college, but also
that the women are more qualified. Karen Cottrell, associate
provost for enrollment at the College of William & Mary
said girls are making the grade. Girls typically have
better high school transcripts, she said. Research and
interviews conducted by USA Today show that even the
most academically talented boys never catch up to girls in
high school grade point averages.
Sociologist Andrew Hacker, author of
Mismatch: The Growing Gulf between Women and
Men, is not surprised by the increased number of women
on college campuses. According to Hacker, three of four high
school senior girls report spending an hour or more on
homework every day, compared to about two out of four boys.
Statistics showed boys report, watching more
television than girls do and spending more time on video
games.
Hacker continues, Its
almost as if being a man and being masculine, macho and
powerful is not conducive to being a good
student.
Michael Thompson, a school
psychologist who wrote Raising Cain on the
academic problems of boys, agreed with Hackers
theory.
Boys hear that the way to shine
is athletically. And boys get a lot of mixed messages about
what it means to be masculine and what it means to be a
student. Does being a good student make you a real man? I
dont think so
It is not cool.
Despite speculation that boys simply
do not try as hard as girls, the bottom line is males are
slowly becoming a minority group on many campuses across the
nation. The more diverse a campus, the more ideas and ways
of thinking are represented in the student. Such a large
female student body can potentially promote a homogenous
population. This poses an interesting question, which most
schools are dealing with quietly. Should males be treated as
a minority, accepting less qualified males over
females?
Most college admissions officers are
reluctant to discuss special preferences boys
applications receive. However, Robert Massa, the director of
admissions at Pennsylvanias Dickinson College, admits
to tilting the admissions scale toward boys. The
male-female ratio at Dickinson is 45-55. Massa said that
without preferences for male applicants, the percentage of
men would drop as low as 38 percent. This is similar to
admissions policies at Wake Forest, in which the school
policy is to maintain 50/50 ratio.
Source: www.elon.edu/e-web/pendulum/issues/2004/3_11/news/gender.xhtml

Sex
differences in academic achievement are not related to
political, economic or social equality
New research has revealed that girls lead boys in
educational achievement in 70% of countries, regardless of
levels of national gender equality.
Even in countries where womens
liberties are severely restricted, girls are shown to be
outperforming boys in mathematics, reading and science
literacy subjects by age 15.
The research, conducted by
psychologists at the universities of Glasgow and Missouri,
looked at the educational achievement levels of 1.5 million
15 year olds from around the world using Programme for
International Student Assessment (PISA) data taken between
2000 and 2010.
The findings, which are published in
the journal Intelligence, show that countries
with high levels of social, political and economic equality
still experience gender differences in academic achievement.
Paradoxically, some of the countries known for relatively
low gender equality ratings, such as Qatar, Jordan and the
United Arab Emirates are those where the educational
achievement gap is relatively large, in favour of
girls.
When considering combined achievements
in mathematics, reading and science, boys fall behind in 70%
of countries, whereas there are only three countries/regions
(4% of those included in the survey) where boys outperform
girls (namely Colombia, Costa Rica, and the Indian state
Himachal Pradesh). The United Kingdom and United States
belonged to the countries where no (statistically
significant) sex difference in combined achievement was
found.
The achievement disparity between boys
and girls is most in evidence at the lowest levels of
achievement. The gap however closes, and is sometimes
reversed, among the very highest achievers in more
economically developed countries.
Researchers claim that these findings
show that policy makers need to look beyond setting targets
for gender equality as a way of reducing gender gaps in
school achievement.
Dr Gijsbert Stoet, Reader in
Psychology at the University of Glasgow, who led the study,
said: The results of this study show that a commitment
towards gender equality on its own is not enough to close
the achievement gaps in global education.
At the moment we see that, with
the exception of high-achievers, boys have poorer
educational outcomes than girls around the world,
independent of social equality indicators. Whats more
is that this gap in not reducing. If policy makers are
seriously concerned about gender equality in education, this
ought to be their top priority. That it is not is probably
fuelled by a lack of public understanding of the
distribution of skills which we have highlighted in this and
previous studies. For example, listening to many news
stories in the media, one can easily get the idea that girls
around the world are falling behind boys, in particular in
countries with known gender inequality. The reality is quite
different in the many countries participating in PISA, which
many may find surprising.
Of course we understand that
there are many reasons beyond education attainment to strive
for gender equality within societies that are not measured
in this study. Although it is vital that we promote gender
equality in schools we also need to make sure that
were doing more to understand why these gaps,
especially the poor achievement of boys, in educational
attainment persist and what other policies we can develop to
close them.
Researchers believe that PISA scores
may also help define why fewer girls, despite being
educationally stronger than boys at 15 years, go on to study
STEM subject at university.
David Geary, Professor of Psychology
at the University of Missouri, said: As well as being
stronger in STEM subjects, the PISA data show that a greater
percentage of girls have proportionally better reading
achievement compared with mathematics achievement, with boys
showing the opposite pattern.
The tilt in skills influences later
choice of college major and occupation so those who are
better at language related skills than maths tend towards
language-based professions, such as law instead of computer
science, even women with very high maths skills. The sex
differences we see in STEM industries today is related in
part to this tilt.
The full paper is available online:
www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289614001688

Sources: Gender equality indexes
referenced in the study include the World Economic
Forums Global Gender Gap Report and the United
Nations Human Development Report. www.gla.ac.uk/news/headline_388432_en.html
Gender gaps among
students revealed by Ucas
Nearly 58,000 more women entered university this academic
year than men, according to new figures that suggest the
academic achievement gap between the sexes is
growing.
Some 285,100 women were accepted on to
higher education courses last autumn compared with just
2,300 men, says the admissions body Ucas in analysis of
university admissions in 2014-15.
The overall difference between women
and men has increased steadily each year from 47,200 in
2008, Ucas says.
Women also achieve higher marks while
at university, according to data published by the Higher
Education Statistics Agency last week.
Seventy-two per cent of women received
at least an upper-second-class degree in 2013-14 compared
with just 67 per cent of men.
The latest Ucas information, which
shows a record 512,000 people were accepted into higher
education last year, also details which subjects are mostly
likely to be dominated by men or women.
Male students strongly outnumber
female ones on engineering and computer science courses,
with 20,300 more men doing engineering and 17,300 more on
computer courses.
However, there are more women than men
in about two-thirds of subjects, in which they outnumber men
by a total of 107,500. These include subjects allied to
medicine and education, where four in five students are
women.
There were between 12,000 and 13,000
more women than men in creative arts and design, education
and social subjects.
The Ucas analysis of 2014-15
admissions also shows the number of students admitted with
vocational BTEC qualifications, rather than just A levels,
has almost doubled since 2008.
Some 85,000 of 369,000 students
admitted to higher education through Ucas main
application scheme held a BTEC, it says.
It means 23.4 per cent of students
entered university in 2014-15 with a BTEC compared with just
13.5 per cent in 2008.
A large majority of universities took
a significant number of students who held BTECs, it
adds.
Only 26 large providers took fewer
than 5 per cent of their intake from BTEC holders in 2014-15
compared with almost double that number (47) in 2008, Ucas
says.
Source: www.timeshighereducation.com/news/gender-gaps-among-students-revealed-by-ucas/2018113.article
Student
suspension from school: Impact on academic achievement by
race
For decades, educators and policymakers have been
concerned about the gaps in academic achievement that have
long separated white children and minority children.
Numerous efforts to reform public education programs have
been launched nationally and locally to try to boost student
test scores and help youth of all races and ethnicities
perform at the same level. While students have made
progress, disparities remain and, in some cases, still are
quite large. For example, white, black and Latino students
in the U.S. all had better high school graduation rates in
2011-12 compared to 2009-10. However, white students were
more likely to graduate. In 2011-12, 85 percent of white
students finished high school on time and with a regular
diploma compared to 68 percent of black students and 76
percent of Latino students, according to data from the
National Center for Education Statistics.
Federal data shows that student scores
on the SAT college-entrance exam have improved as well. The
combined math and reading scores of white students grew from
an average score of 1038 in 1986-87 to a score of 1061 in
2012-13. Over the same time period, black students raised
their combined score from an 839, on average, to a 956.
Puerto Rican students went from a score of 868 to a 909 and
Mexican American students scores rose one point to a
913.
As the nation scrutinizes the academic
performance of minority children, scholars and researchers
have been looking into the factors that could be hindering
children of color. One area of focus is instruction time
whether minority children have less time in the
classroom because of illness, absenteeism, tardiness or
disciplinary actions such as suspensions or expulsions. A
September 2015 report released by Attendance Works and the
Healthy Schools Campaign looks at how chronic absenteeism
disproportionately affects children from minority and
low-income families and those with disabilities. Several
studies in recent years have taken on the issue of student
suspension, revealing alarming trends. For example, a
92-page report from the University of Pennsylvanias
Graduate School of Education found that 55 percent of
black-student suspensions nationwide in 2011-12 occurred in
13 Southern states. In 84 Southern school districts, black
students were the only students who were suspended that
school year.
A 2016 study published in Social
Problems contributes new insights to the ongoing discussion
about racial disparities in school discipline and academic
achievement. The authors of the study, titled The
Punishment Gap: School Suspension and Racial Disparities in
Achievement, indicate that it is the first-ever
comprehensive analysis of suspension as an explanation for
racial gaps in student performance in math and reading. The
authors Edward W. Morris of the University of
Kentucky and Brea L. Perry of Indiana University used
data from the Kentucky School Discipline Study and school
records to examine how suspension affected a sample of
16,248 students in grades 6 through 10 over a three-year
period. Most of the students involved 59 percent
were white while 25 percent were black and 10 percent
were Latino. Four percent of students were Asian and 3
percent identified as being another race.
The studys key findings
include:
- Schools with larger concentrations
of black students had higher rates of
suspension.
- Black students and Latino students
were more likely to be suspended than children from other
racial groups.
- Students who had been suspended
earned significantly lower scores in math and reading on
end-of-year exams. Students with a propensity to be
suspended did worse on the exam during the years they
were suspended than during years they were
not.
- Students who qualified for free or
reduced-price lunches at school were more likely to be
suspended than those who did not. Students who
participated in special-education programs were more
likely to be suspended. Students with two parents were
less likely to be suspended than those with one parent or
guardian.
- Even after controlling for
socioeconomic status, special education and gender, black
students were predicted to be almost three times more
likely to be suspended than white students. On the other
hand, the elevated risk of suspension associated
with being Latino is entirely explained by this
groups lower levels of socioeconomic
status.
The authors state that while their
findings suggest a strong link between suspension and lower
academic achievement, they cannot prove that suspension
causes poorer test scores. The authors suggest that future
research should aim to assess whether other acts of
discipline are associated with reduced achievement. They
also recommend trying to determine whether the suspension or
the missed class time is what underlies the connection
between suspension and achievement.
Related research: A 2014
research roundup explores the strategies that schools use to
eliminate or prevent violence on campus. A 2014 study
published in the American Educational Research Journal,
Parsing Disciplinary Disproportionality: Contributions
of Infraction, Student and School Characteristics to
Out-of-School Suspension and Expulsion, looks at how
rates of suspension and expulsion can be predicted by the
type of infraction committed as well as demographic factors
and principals attitudes. A 2014 study from scholars
at Johns Hopkins University, Sent Home and Put
Off-Track: The Antecedents, Disproportionalities, and
Consequences of Being Suspended in the Ninth Grade,
looks at how suspending students during their first year of
high school can influence their academic performance and the
likelihood they will attend college.
Source: journalistsresource.org/studies/society/education/race-school-suspension-academic-achievement
Parents aiming
too high can harm child's academic performance
Aspiration can help academic achievement only if it is
realistic
Summary:
When parents have high hopes for their
children's academic achievement, the children tend to do
better in school, unless those hopes are unrealistic, in
which case the children may not perform well in
school.
When parents have high hopes for their
children's academic achievement, the children tend to do
better in school, unless those hopes are unrealistic, in
which case the children may not perform well in school,
according to research published by the American
Psychological Association.
"Our research revealed both positive
and negative aspects of parents' aspiration for their
children's academic performance. Although parental
aspiration can help improve children's academic performance,
excessive parental aspiration can be poisonous," said lead
author Kou Murayama, PhD, of the University of Reading. The
study was published in the Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology.
Murayama and his colleagues analyzed
data from a longitudinal study from 2002 to 2007 of 3,530
secondary school students (49.7 percent female) and their
parents in Bavaria, Germany. The study assessed student math
achievement as well as parental aspiration (how much they
want their child to earn a particular grade) and expectation
(how much they believe their child can achieve a certain
grade) on an annual basis.
They found that high parental
aspiration led to increased academic achievement, but only
when it did not overly exceed realistic expectation. When
aspiration exceeded expectation, the children's achievement
decreased proportionately.
To reinforce the results, the
researchers attempted to replicate the main findings of the
study using data from a two-year study of over 12,000 U.S.
students and their parents. The results were similar to the
German study and provided further evidence that parents'
overly high aspirations are associated with worse academic
performance by their kids.
Previous psychological research has
found the association between aspiration and academic
achievement, but this study highlights a caveat, said
Murayama.
"Much of the previous literature
conveyed a simple, straightforward message to parents -- aim
high for your children and they will achieve more," said
Murayama. In fact, getting parents to have higher hopes for
their children has often been a goal of programs designed to
improve academic performance in schools. This study suggests
that the focus of such educational programs should not be on
blindly increasing parental aspiration but on giving parents
the information they need to develop realistic
expectations.
"Unrealistically high aspiration may
hinder academic performance. Simply raising aspiration
cannot be an effective solution to improve success in
education," he said.
Source: www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/11/151117112652.htm
Four-Day
School Week Can Improve Academic Performance, Policy Study
Finds
Shortening the school week to four days has a positive
impact on elementary school students academic
performance in mathematics, according to researchers at
Georgia State University and Montana State
University.
The study, published in the journal
Education, Finance and Policy in July, analyzed the impact
of a four-day school week on student achievement by
comparing fourth-grade reading and fifth-grade math test
scores from the Colorado Student Assessment Program (CSAP)
for students who participated in a four-day school week,
versus those who attended a traditional five-day school
week.
The researchers found a four-day
school week had a statistically significant impact on math
scores for fifth-grade students, while reading scores were
not affected.
The study suggests there is little
evidence that moving to a four-day week compromises student
academic achievement, an important finding for U.S. school
districts seeking ways to cut costs without hampering
student achievement.
What interested me about our
results is they were completely opposite to what we
anticipated, said Mary Beth Walker, dean of the Andrew
Young School of Policy Studies at Georgia State. We
thought that especially for the younger, elementary school
kids, longer days on a shorter school week would hurt their
academic performance because their attention spans are
shorter. Also, a longer weekend would give them more
opportunity to forget what they had
learned.
Although the shortened school week did
not have a measurable impact on reading outcomes, the
idea that the change in the calendar did not have negative
effects we thought was an important result, Walker
said.
A number of school districts in the
United States have moved from the traditional Monday through
Friday schedule to a four-day week schedule as a cost-saving
measure to reduce overhead and transportation
costs.
Four-day weeks have been in place for
years in rural school districts in western states,
particularly in Colorado, New Mexico and Wyoming. Over
one-third of the school districts in Colorado have adopted a
four-day schedule. The alternative schedule has also been
considered in school districts in Oregon, Missouri, Florida
and Georgia.
The four-day school week requires
school districts to lengthen the school day to meet minimum
instructional hour requirements. Previously, there was a
lack of information on whether the four-day school week
affects student performance, Walker said.
The researchers have speculated on why
the shortened school week positively affected students but
there are not enough data to draw definite
conclusions.
We thought the longer days might
give teachers an opportunity to use different kinds of
instructional processes, Walker said. We also
speculated that a four-day school week lowered absenteeism,
so students who had dentists appointments or events
might be able to put those off until Friday and not miss
school. We thought there might be less teacher
absenteeism.
My own personal hypothesis is
teachers liked it so muchthey were so enthusiastic
about the four-day weekthey did a better job.
Theres some evidence in other labor studies that
four-day work weeks enhance productivity.
Walker notes the results are only
applicable to smaller and more rural school districts.
Further studies should be performed to understand the
effects on urban school districts, she said.
Read the
study
Source: news.gsu.edu/2015/08/27/four-day-school-week-can-improve-academic-performance-policy-study-finds/
Gender
differences in school achievement: The role of
self-regulation
Abstract
This study examined whether different
aspects of self-regulation (i.e., emotion and behavior
regulation) account for gender differences in German and
mathematics achievement. Specifically, we investigated
whether higher school achievement by girls in comparison to
boys can be explained by self-regulation. German and
mathematics achievement were assessed in a sample of 53
German fifth graders (19 boys, 34 girls) using formal
academic performance tests (i.e., reading, writing,
mathematics) and teachers' ratings (i.e., grades in German
and mathematics). Moreover, teachers rated children's
behavior regulation using the Self-Control Scale (SCS-K-D).
Children's self-reported strategies of emotion regulation
were assessed with the Questionnaire for the Measurement of
Stress and Coping in Children and Adolescents (SSKJ 3-8).
Age and intelligence (CFT 20-R) were included as control
variables. Analyses of mean differences showed that girls
outperformed boys in German achievement and behavior
regulation. Regression analyses, using a bootstrapping
method, revealed that relations between gender and German
achievement were mediated by behavior regulation.
Furthermore, we found a suppression effect of behavior
regulation on the relation between gender and mathematics
achievement: boys' mathematics achievement was
underestimated when the analyses did not control for
behavior regulation. We discuss these results from a
developmental perspective and within the theoretical
framework of self-regulation and achievement.
Currently, both scientific literature
and German mass media are discussing the discrepancy in
school achievement between boys and girls, going so far as
to call boys the new losers of the educational system
(Spiewak, 2010, August 5). Several studies have found
significant gender differences in school achievement
favoring girls over boys (Cole, 1997; Duckworth and
Seligman, 2006). According to the German census, there are
more girls than boys in higher secondary schools, whereas
more boys than girls attend lower secondary schools. As a
consequence, more girls achieve the general qualification
for university entrance, whereas more boys complete the
certificate of lower secondary school (Statistisches
Bundesamt, 2011).
The reasons for these gender
differences in school achievement have not been clarified
yet. Past research has shown that besides cognitive
abilities (e.g., intelligence; Deary et al., 2007; Spinath
et al., 2010) the motivation and ability to self-regulate is
positively associated with school achievement (Duckworth and
Seligman, 2005; Suchodoletz et al., 2009). In line with
these findings, previous studies have indicated that
specific components of self-regulationbehavioral
regulation or self-regulated learningcould contribute
to gender differences in school achievement (Duckworth and
Seligman, 2006; Kuhl and Hannover, 2012). However, by only
investigating behavior regulation, these previous studies
neglected the wider conceptualization of self-regulation.
The concept of self-regulation includes both behavior
regulation and emotion regulation, and both aspects of
self-regulation may be related to children's school
achievement (Blair, 2002; Calkins, 2007; McClelland et al.,
2007). Therefore, it is important to understand the
contribution of behavior and emotion regulation to gender
differences in school achievement.
In the present study, we investigated
in a sample of German fifth graders who had just
transitioned from primary school to secondary school whether
self-regulation mediates effects of gender on school
achievement. In particular, we studied the relations between
different aspects of self-regulation (i.e., behavior
regulation, emotion regulation) and school achievement in
different domains (i.e., German and mathematics
achievement).
Past research suggested that girls are
in general more successful in school than boys. Hartley and
Sutton (2013) have recently reported that especially boys
develop gender stereotypes according to which girls are
perceived as academically superior with regard to
motivation, ability, performance, and self-regulation.
However, previous studies revealed rather inconsistent
results concerning gender differences in different domains
of school achievement. In the present study, we focused on
achievement in German and mathematics because performance in
these subjects is seen as an important aspect of school
achievement (Schrader and Helmke, 2008). Previous
large-scale studies revealed higher German achievement by
girls in comparison to boys (Stanat and Kunter, 2003; Stanat
et al., 2012). However, the picture of gender differences in
mathematics achievement is less clear (Hannover and Kessels,
2011; Stanat et al., 2012). While in some studies boys
exceeded girls in mathematics achievement, in other studies
no gender differences in mathematics achievement were found
(Hannover and Kessels, 2011). For instance, Machin and
Pekkarinen (2008) argued that mixed evidence for gender
differences in school achievement could be explained in part
by a higher variance of boys' in comparison to girls' school
achievement.
As Hyde (1990) pointed out,
meta-analyses have consistently shown that there are no
significant gender differences in general cognitive
abilities. Thus, although cognitive abilities are
significantly and positively related to school achievement,
they cannot explain gender differences in school achievement
(Spinath et al., 2010). Therefore, further
non-cognitive variables have been examined in an
attempt to explain gender differences in school achievement.
For instance, Spinath et al. (2010) highlighted the
importance of personality and motivation for gender
differences in school achievement. They found that a higher
level of extraversion was associated with higher grades for
girls but lower grades for boys. Pomerantz et al. (2002)
noted that girls want to please adults to a higher degree
than do boys, which leads to girls' higher school grades.
Furthermore, stereotypes are an important influence on
school achievement in that negative stereotypes disrupt
girls' mathematics performance (e.g., Keller and
Dauenheimer, 2003). However, a rarely considered explanation
for gender differences in school achievement from a
developmental point of view is self-regulation (Duckworth
and Seligman, 2006).
Self-regulation and school
achievement
Various terms and definitions have
been used to conceptualize self-regulation and its
components (McClelland et al., 2010). Here, self-regulation
is understood as the motivation and ability to maintain
goal-directed actions over time and across several
situational contexts in order to achieve desired goals
(Karoly, 1993). Although relatively stable differences exist
between individuals with regard to the motivation and
ability to self-regulate (Raffaelli et al., 2005), there is
situation specific variance in self-regulation within
individuals depending on domain-specific temptation
(Tsukayama et al., 2012). Self-regulation is conceived as a
broad construct which includes the more specific components
behavior regulation and emotion regulation. Behavior
regulation includes the motivation and ability to pay
attention, to follow rules, to resist temptation, and to
inhibit inappropriate actions (e.g., McClelland et al.,
2007; Heikamp et al., 2013). In contrast, emotion regulation
is a process that serves to initiate, to inhibit, to
maintain, or to modulate the experiences of emotions in
order to achieve social adaptation or individual goals
(Eisenberg and Spinrad, 2004). In the present study, we
focused on strategies of emotion regulation that aim to
change the experience of negative emotions (Cole et al.,
2004). According to the transactional model of stress and
coping, problem-oriented and emotion-oriented strategies can
be distinguished (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984).
Problem-oriented strategies are directed to the context and
aim to change a situation that elicited negative emotions.
In contrast, emotion-oriented strategies aim to regulate
emotional experiences by changing the appraisal of a
situation. Whereas problem-oriented strategies include
instrumental actions that aim to change the cause of the
negative emotional experience, emotion-oriented strategies
involve the behavioral and cognitive avoidance of the
problem (Lohaus et al., 2006; Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck,
2007). Behavior regulation and emotion regulation can be
seen as two distinct components of self-regulation. Even
though behavior and emotion regulation are distinguishable
concepts, they are interrelated during the course of
development (Raffaelli et al., 2005). Considering the broad
conceptualization of self-regulation and taking into account
that self-regulation is a multidimensional construct (e.g.,
Duckworth and Kern, 2011), it is important to take a more
nuanced perspective on self-regulation by viewing behavior
regulation and emotion regulation as interrelated but
separate aspects of self-regulation.
The transition from elementary to
secondary school is associated with increasing demands such
as self-organization, homework, and exam preparation in
various subjects. Hence, children need to adopt
self-regulated learning strategies (through goal-setting,
strategy use, and self-monitoring) to be successful in
school (Blair, 2002). Students have to develop
self-regulation strategies, which include goal oriented
processes that aim to regulate emotions and behavior in
order to adapt successfully to school (Zimmerman, 1990;
Schunk and Zimmerman, 1997; Suchodoletz et al., 2009).
Self-regulation, with its components behavior regulation and
emotion regulation, is positively associated with school
achievement (Calkins, 2007; McClelland et al., 2007).
According to Zimmerman and Schunk (2011) self-regulated
students are effective in school because they set learning
goals, apply effective learning strategies, monitor their
own goal progress, establish a productive learning
environment, and develop self-efficacy beliefs for
learning.
Behavior regulation enables one to
remember and follow instructions and to concentrate on tasks
without getting distracted. Therefore, behavior regulation
is positively related to the development of positive
classroom behavior and academic achievement (McClelland et
al., 2007). Most notably, behavior regulation accounts for
additional variance in school achievement above and beyond
the variance that is explained by intelligence (e.g.,
Duckworth and Seligman, 2005; Suchodoletz et al.,
2009).
Blair (2002) argued that adequate
emotion regulation in the classroom facilitates cognitive
processes (e.g., memory, attention, planning, problem
solving), which are necessary for scholastic learning. In
the school context, emotions have to be regulated to allow
for the child's appropriate achievement behavior
(Trommsdorff, in press). In general, both problem-oriented
and emotion-oriented strategies can be adaptive strategies
to regulate emotions. It depends on the situation which
strategy brings higher benefits (Lohaus et al., 2006).
Adaptive emotion regulation means to adopt strategies
flexible depending on the situation (Lohaus et al., 2006;
Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007). Regarding strategies
which are used to regulate negative emotions in the school
context, studies have shown that problem-oriented strategies
have positive effects whereas emotion-oriented strategies
(e.g., avoidance, distraction) have negative effects on
school achievement (e.g., Brdar et al., 2006; Cohen et al.,
2008). This effect can be seen in individual differences in
preparing for examinations and in different relations with
achievement in the school context. For instance, students
who are more likely to use problem-oriented strategies
prepare for examinations and plan their work, whereas
students who use emotion-oriented strategies do not actively
cope with the future examination and thus do not take enough
time to study (Zeidner, 1995). Whereas problem-oriented
strategies might be more effective for school achievement,
emotion-oriented strategies might be adaptive in order to
regulate emotions in the short term (e.g., to feel good) but
may have negative consequences regarding school achievement
in the long run.
Gender, self-regulation, and school
achievement
Bjorklund and Kipp (1996) argue that a
greater evolutionary necessity of women to control their
emotional and behavioral reactions in social situations has
led to women's higher self-regulation abilities. Davis
(1995) suggested that girls are more expected than boys to
act according to social rules, which induces girls having
more practice and therefore a better ability to regulate
their behaviors and emotions. In line with this view,
meta-analytic studies have shown that girls have a higher
motivation and ability to engage in behavior regulation than
boys (e.g., Silverman, 2003; Else-Quest et al., 2006; Cross
et al., 2011). Gender differences have also been reported
with regard to the habitual use of emotion regulation
strategies. For instance, girls tend to use strategies that
aim to solve a problem in order to feel better (i.e.,
problem-oriented strategies) more often than do boys. In
contrast, boys tend to emotionally disengage from stressful
situations (i.e., emotion-oriented strategies) more often
than do girls (Eschenbeck et al., 2007).
Because (a) there is evidence for
greater school achievement and self-regulation by girls and
(b) self-regulation is positively related to school
achievement, one may ask whether self-regulation accounts
for gender differences in school achievement. In a sample of
US-American eighth graders, Duckworth and Seligman (2006)
found that girls' higher school achievement can be explained
in part by behavior regulation. Kuhl and Hannover (2012)
showed that in a sample of German fourth graders, teachers'
ratings of children's self-regulated learning could partly
explain gender differences in school achievement. Here, we
examined both behavior regulation and emotion regulation as
aspects of self-regulation. We investigated whether the
relation between gender and school achievement (German and
mathematics) is mediated by self-regulation (behavior
regulation and emotion regulation). Further, we extended the
mediation models by controlling for age and
intelligence.
Study aims
The present research aimed to test if
gender differences in school achievement can be explained by
gender differences in self-regulation. Therefore, two
mediation models were tested to investigate whether behavior
regulation and emotion regulation mediate the association
between gender and school achievement in German and
mathematics. In line with previous findings (e.g., Cole,
1997; Duckworth and Seligman, 2006), we hypothesized that
girls have greater school achievement than do boys. Building
on past research on gender-differences in behavior
regulation (e.g., Silverman, 2003; Else-Quest et al., 2006;
Cross et al., 2011), we expected that girls show a higher
motivation and ability for behavior regulation than boys.
Regarding gender differences in emotion regulation, we
hypothesized that girls show problem-oriented strategies
more often than boys, whereas boys show emotion-oriented
strategies more often than girls (Eschenbeck et al., 2007).
In order to extend the scope of previous studies, we
examined whether different aspects of self-regulation (i.e.,
emotion and behavior regulation) account for gender
differences in school achievement. Based on past findings,
we expected that the relations between gender and school
achievement are mediated by behavior regulation (Duckworth
and Seligman, 2006; Kuhl and Hannover, 2012). In extension
of past research, we investigated whether there is an
indirect effect of gender on school achievement mediated by
children's use of emotion regulation strategies (i.e.,
problem-oriented strategies, emotion-oriented
strategies).
Materials and
methods
Participants
Fifty-seven children participated in
the study in summer 2010. The children attended 22 different
fifth grade classes in seven different schools in a town in
Southern Germany. The class teachers of the 22 fifth grade
classes were asked to complete questionnaires about those
children of their class who took part in the study. Number
of students for whom each class teacher provided reports of
grades and behavior regulation ranged from 1 to 5. Four
children were excluded from data analysis because of
incomplete data sets. Hence, the sample consisted of 53
fifth graders (34 girls) and their class teachers.
Children's mean age was 11.23 years (SD = .54). Twenty-two
(100%) class teachers (16 female, 6 male) completed
questionnaires about the school achievement (i.e., grades)
and behavior regulation of those students who attended their
class. Thirty-nine (74%) mothers completed questionnaires on
their highest school graduation. Of the mothers, 2 (4%) had
a lower secondary school certificate (= 1), 11 (21%) had a
middle secondary school certificate (= 2), 3 (6%) had a
qualification for university of applied sciences (= 3), and
23 (43%) had a general qualification for university entrance
(= 4). Thus, mother's mean level of education was 3.21 (SD =
1.03). Parents of child participants provided written
informed consent prior to participation. Children who
participated received a 15 € gift card, teachers received a
2.50 € gift card for every child they evaluated (15 €
maximum), and mothers who answered the questionnaire
received a 7 € gift card.
Procedure
In summer of 2010, fifth graders
participated at two group-sessions (up to 10 children) in
rooms of the university. Each session lasted about 2 h and
consisted of two parts (computer lab and seminar room)
separated by a 10 min break. Questionnaires and standardized
tests were administered in group sessions, limited to 10
children per session. The first session included the
nonverbal intelligence test, the mathematics achievement
test, and questionnaires. In the second session, reading and
writing skills and further questionnaires were administered
because the present study was part of a larger project on
the relations between self-regulation and school
achievement. Teachers and mothers answered paper-and-pencil
questionnaires at home.
Materials
Assessment of school
achievement
In order to measure school
achievement, grades as well as standardized reading,
writing, and mathematics tests were assessed. German and
mathematics grades were assessed by teachers' reports.
Grades were based on children's classroom work and grades of
class examinations in the first half of fifth grade (i.e.,
fifth grade midterm report). School grades were recoded in a
way such that a higher score indicated higher school
achievement (i.e., 1 = not sufficient/fail to 6 = very
good). According to the German curriculum, German grades
reflect, besides reading and writing skills, language
proficiency (e.g., understanding the meaning of texts and
reflection of language use) as well as communication and
speech competencies (e.g., presentation of texts, written
and oral expression; e.g., Ministerium für Kultus,
Jugend und Sport Baden-Württemberg, 2004). Basic
reading skills were assessed by measuring reading speed
using the Salzburger Reading-Screening for 5th to 8th
graders (Auer et al., 2008). Writing skills were measured
with the Hamburger Writing Test (May, 2007), which consists
of a text with mistakes to be corrected. This test assesses
the number of corrected words and punctuation marks and
provides an individual profile of orthography strategies.
The mathematics subtests numerical comprehension,
calculation, and quantities from the Hamburger school
achievement test for 4th and 5th graders (Mietzel and
Willenberg, 2000) was used in order to assess children's
mathematics performance. To avoid influences of confounding
variables (e.g., stereotype threat) reading, writing, and
mathematics tests were conducted in a standardized manner,
following the instructions of the manuals. As aggregated
measures combining grades and standardized school
achievement tests are more valid measures than separate
measures (e.g., teachers' perceptions of children's
characteristics can be related with school grades; Mullola
et al., 2010), correlations were computed to test whether
grades and test scores are significantly related. Pearson
correlations showed significantly positive correlations of
German grades to reading skills (r = .33, p < .05) and to
writing skills (r = .37, p < .01) and between test
performance in mathematics and mathematics grades (r = .48,
p < .01). Test scores and school grades were standardized
by computing z-scores and mean scores were computed for
German and mathematics achievement. Accordingly, reading and
writing skills and German grades were averaged into a German
achievement score. Mathematics test performance and
mathematics grades were averaged into a mathematics
achievement score.
Assessment of
self-regulation
In order to assess individual
differences in behavior regulation, the German version of
the widely used, reliable, and valid Self-Control Scale
(Tangney et al., 2004) from Bertrams and Dickhäuser
(2009) was administered. Class teachers answered the 13
items on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all to 5 = very much),
e.g., The child has a hard time breaking bad
habits.. Reliability analysis revealed a Cronbach's a
of .94 in the present study.
Strategies of emotion regulation
(i.e., problem- and emotion-oriented strategies) were
measured using the Questionnaire for the Measurement of
Stress and Coping in Children and Adolescents (SSKJ 3-8)
(Lohaus et al., 2006). In this questionnaire, children are
asked to think of a situation in which they have problems
doing their homework. Children answered the items on a
5-point rating scale (from 1 = never to 5 = always) by
indicating how often they use problem-oriented strategies (6
items; e.g., I try to think of different ways to solve
it.) and emotion-oriented strategies (6 items, e.g.,
I tell myself it doesn't matter.) to cope with
their emotions. Reliability analyses revealed a Cronbach's a
of .80 for problem-oriented strategies and a Cronbach's a of
.75 for emotion-oriented strategies.
Assessment of
intelligence
In order to assess nonverbal
intelligence, the short version of the CFT 20-R (Weiß,
2006) was administered. Sum scores were transformed into
age-standardized IQ scores.
Data analysis
Pearson correlations were computed to
investigate associations of intelligence, age, and mother's
level of education with self-regulation (i.e., behavior
regulation, emotion regulation) and school achievement
(i.e., German and mathematics achievement). Multivariate
analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) were computed in order to
test gender differences in school achievement (i.e., German
and mathematics achievement) and self-regulation (i.e.,
emotion and behavior regulation). Mediation models were
tested by using the bootstrapping method by Preacher and
Hayes (2008). Besides the fact that a bootstrapping approach
is especially suitable for small sample sizes, this
procedure has two strengths compared to conventional methods
of mediation tests. First, multiple mediators are tested in
the same model at the same time. Second, using bootstrapping
avoids the assumption of a normal distribution of the
indirect effects. For estimating point estimates, 5000
bootstrap samples were drawn and, for the indirect effects,
95% confidence intervals were used. A post-hoc power
analysis was conducted to analyze, if the sample size was
big enough to detect significant mediation effects (Faul et
al., 2007)1.
Results
Descriptive statistics are shown in
Table ?Table1.1. In general, boys and girls in the sample
had good school achievement, as shown by their grades as
well as standardized reading, writing, and mathematics
tests. On average, teachers rated children's behavior
regulation as high. Overall, boys and girls rated themselves
as using problem-oriented strategies more often than
emotion-oriented strategies. Children's nonverbal
intelligence and mothers' level of education were slightly
above average.
Table 1
Descriptive
statistics.
Pearson correlations revealed that age
was significantly negatively correlated with intelligence
and German achievement. Perhaps older children had lower
nonverbal IQ and academic abilities because they already had
to repeat school grades. Nonverbal intelligence correlated
significantly and positively with German and mathematics
achievement. No significant relations were found between
mother's level of education and self-regulation (i.e.,
behavior regulation, problem- and emotion-oriented
strategies of emotion regulation) or school-achievement
variables (i.e., German and mathematics achievement) (see
Table ?Table2).2). Consequently, age and intelligence were
entered as control variables in further analyses.
Table 2
Pearson correlation
matrix.
Separate MANCOVAs were conducted to
test gender differences in school achievement (i.e., German
and mathematics achievement) and in self-regulation (i.e.,
behavior regulation, problem- and emotion-oriented
strategies of emotion regulation). In both MANCOVAs age and
intelligence were included as covariates. Using a Bonferroni
adjusted alpha level of .025, the MANCOVA revealed
significant gender differences in German achievement
favoring girls, F(1, 49) = 5.90, p = .019, ?2 = .11, but no
significant gender differences in mathematics achievement
F(1, 49) = 1.16, p = .287, ?2 = .02. The MANCOVA regarding
gender differences in self-regulation (i.e., behavior
regulation, problem- and emotion-oriented strategies of
emotion regulation) using a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level
of .017, revealed a significant gender effect for behavior
regulation favoring girls, F(1, 49) = 6.65, p = .013, ?2 =
.12. However, there were no significant gender effects with
regard to problem-oriented strategies, F(1, 49) = .14, p =
.706, ?2 = .00 or emotion-oriented strategies, F(1, 49) =
.01, p = .918, ?2 = .00. The means and standard deviations
for school achievement and the self-regulation variables are
shown in Table ?Table33.
Table 3
Summary statistics for school
achievement and self-regulation variables.
Further, we tested whether gender
differences in children's school achievement were mediated
by self-regulation (i.e., behavior regulation, problem and
emotion-oriented strategies of emotion regulation).
Therefore, two multiple mediation models were tested
separately. In one model, German achievement was regarded as
a dependent variable and, in the other model, mathematics
achievement was regarded as a dependent variable. In both
models, age and intelligence were included as control
variables. Indirect effects are unstandardized coefficients,
which are significant when the 95% confident interval does
not contain zero.
The relations between gender,
self-regulation (i.e., behavior regulation, problem- and
emotion-oriented strategies of emotion regulation), and
school achievement, controlled for age and intelligence, are
presented in Figure ?Figure1.1. Behavior regulation was
significantly and positively related to German and
mathematics achievement. Problem-oriented strategies were
neither significantly associated with German achievement nor
mathematics achievement. Emotion-oriented strategies were
significantly and negatively related to German achievement
but not significantly associated with mathematics
achievement.
Figure 1
Multiple mediation tests of the
relations of gender to German and mathematics achievement
mediated by behavior regulation and strategies of emotion
regulation. Multiple mediation test of the relation between
gender and German achievement mediated by behavior
...
Figure ?Figure1A1A shows the results
of the mediation model with gender as an independent
variable; behavior regulation, problem-oriented strategies,
and emotion-oriented strategies as mediator variables;
German achievement as the dependent variable; and age and
intelligence as covariates. The total effect c was
significant, while the direct effect c' was non-significant.
Behavior regulation significantly mediated the relation
between gender and German achievement (indirect effect =
.226, SE = .116, 95% CI [.056, .541]). Behavior
regulation was a significant mediator because its 95%
confidence interval did not contain zero. Neither
problem-oriented strategies nor emotion-oriented strategies
were significant mediators (for problem-oriented strategies:
indirect effect = .009, SE = .036, 95% CI [-.037,
.126]; for emotion-oriented strategies: indirect effect
= .007, SE = .063, 95% CI [-.119, .144]; see Figure
?Figure1A1A).
Figure ?Figure1B1B shows the results
of the mediation model with gender as an independent
variable; behavior regulation, problem-oriented strategies
and emotion-oriented strategies as mediator variables;
mathematics achievement as the dependent variable; and age
and intelligence as covariates. The total effect c was not
significant, whereas the direct effect c' was significantly
negative. This means, there was no significant gender
difference in mathematics achievement (total effect c) but,
when self-regulation variables were entered in the model,
there was a significant direct effect (c') of gender on
mathematics favoring boys. Thereby, there was a significant
indirect effect of gender on mathematics achievement through
behavior regulation (indirect effect = .258, SE = .142, 95%
CI [.057, .611]). Hence, there was a suppression
effect of behavior regulation on the relation between gender
and mathematics achievement. Neither the indirect effect of
problem-oriented strategies nor the indirect effect of
emotion-oriented strategies were significant (for
problem-oriented strategies: indirect effect = .008, SE =
.010, 95% CI [-.051, .130]; for emotion-oriented
strategies: indirect effect = -.001, SE = .026, 95% CI
[-.079, .038]; see Figure ?Figure1B1B).
Discussion
As hypothesized, the present study
revealed that German achievement was higher for girls than
for boys. There were no gender differences in mathematics
achievement. These results are consistent with the results
of some studies in the literature, which have also found
higher achievement in German or in other language subjects
(e.g., English) by girls but no significant gender
differences in mathematics achievement (e.g., Spinath et
al., 2010; Kuhl and Hannover, 2012). Extending previous
research, we investigated gender differences in German and
mathematics achievement taking children's motivation and
ability for emotion and behavior regulation into
account.
The results of the present study
revealed that gender differences in German achievement were
explained by gender differences in behavior regulation. This
finding emphasizes the central function of behavior
regulation for German achievement in general as well as the
function of behavior regulation for gender differences in
German achievement. The interpretation of the results
regarding mathematics achievement is more complicated. There
was no conventional mediation effect of behavior regulation
on the relation between gender and mathematics achievement.
Surprisingly, an interesting suppression effect occurred.
There was a significant indirect effect of behavior
regulation by gender on mathematics achievement. This means
that the mathematics achievement of boys is underestimated
when analyses do not control for behavior
regulation.
The suppression effect could be a
reason for the inconsistent findings regarding gender
differences in mathematics achievement. The gender
difference in mathematics achievement favoring boys is not
found when analyses do not control for behavior regulation
because girls' higher behavior regulation and the positive
effect of behavior regulation on mathematics achievement
cancel each other out. This finding could explain why some
studies find gender differences in mathematics achievement
whereas others do not, as shown in the overview by Hannover
and Kessels (2011). There might be other variables that
moderate the indirect effect of gender on mathematics
achievement. For instance, if girls are confronted with
negative stereotypes about females' mathematics achievement,
their mathematics achievement worsens (e.g., Keller and
Dauenheimer, 2003). A recent study by Galdi et al. (2013)
has shown that even when girls are not aware of the
mathematics-gender stereotype, automatic associations
consistent with the stereotype may hinder girls' mathematics
achievement. Hence, for girls with strong negative
stereotypes about their mathematics achievement or with the
presence of stereotype-consistent automatic associations,
behavior regulation might be less strongly related to girls'
mathematics achievement in comparison to girls with less
negative gender stereotypes. In this case, gender
differences in mathematics achievement, favoring boys can be
found. Without the presence of stereotypes or
stereotype-consistent automatic associations, no gender
differences in mathematics achievement would be found
because of the suppression effect of behavior regulation. In
contrast to former studies, in addition to behavior
regulation, we examined the role of emotion regulation on
gender differences in school achievement. The present study
revealed that strategies of emotion regulation (i.e.,
problem- and emotion-oriented strategies of emotion
regulation) did not mediate the relation between gender and
school achievement. As post-hoc power analyses revealed low
power for detecting small and medium effects, future studies
with larger samples and higher power may find significant
mediation effects of emotion regulation strategies.
Nevertheless, the present study revealed a significant and
negative relation between the use of emotion-oriented
strategies of emotion regulation and German achievement.
This result suggests that children who tend to engage in
active coping are more likely to show higher German
achievement than children who tend to disengage mentally and
behaviorally from stressful school-related situations (e.g.,
a lot of homework).
Strengths and
limitations
Although the sample size was rather
small and children came from a rather homogeneous
middle-class socio-economic background, analyses revealed
significant gender differences in behavior regulation and
German achievement. For instance, gender accounted for a
substantial amount of variance in behavior regulation (12%)
and German achievement (11%). However, future research using
larger and more diverse samples is desirable in order to be
able to generalize the findings of the present study to
larger populations. Furthermore, emotion regulation was
assessed by children's self-reports only. Further studies
should include a direct measure of emotion regulation as
well as a multiple-measure strategy that takes also other
strategies of emotion regulation into account (e.g.,
reappraisal; Gross and Thompson, 2007). In addition, the
present study relied on class teachers' reports for the
assessment of children's behavior regulation. Ideally, to
measure behavior regulation, direct and multiple-measure
strategies should be used. It should also be noted that
school grades are teacher evaluations, too. In order to take
these limitations into account school achievement was
assessed by school grades (i.e., mid-term report grades in
German and mathematics) and by standardized achievement
tests. Moreover, children's self-regulation (i.e., behavior
regulation, emotion regulation) was assessed by teacher
report and a self-report measure.
Theoretical
implications
In line with previous results, the
present study revealed that German achievement and the
motivation and ability for behavior regulation was higher
for girls than for boys. Moreover, indirect effects of
gender on German and mathematics achievement were mediated
by children's behavior regulation, but not by strategies of
emotion regulation. Furthermore, mediation analyses
indicated that mathematics achievement was higher for boys
than for girls. However, gender differences in mathematics
achievement were canceled out because of girls' higher
motivation and ability for behavior regulation that was
positively associated with mathematics achievement. Hence,
further studies analyzing gender differences in mathematics
achievement should consider the possibility that the
mathematics achievement of boys may be underestimated when
not controlling for behavior regulation. Further studies
should investigate whether variables such as stereotype
threat moderate relations between gender, behavior
regulation, and mathematics achievement. Moreover, as
culture influences the development of self-regulation
(Trommsdorff, 2009; Heikamp et al., 2013), longitudinal
studies are needed to draw causal conclusions concerning the
effect of socialization in different contexts (e.g.,
culture, family, school) on the development of gender
differences in self-regulation and school
achievement.
Conflict of interest
statement
The authors declare that the research
was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial
relationships that could be construed as a potential
conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This research was financed by a grant
from the German Research Foundation (DFG GZ, TR 169/14-3) to
the last author. This study is part of the project
Developmental Conditions of Intentionality and its
Limits (Principal Investigator: Gisela Trommsdorff)
within the interdisciplinary research group Limits of
Intentionality (FOR 582) at the University of
Konstanz, Germany.
Footnotes
1For the statistical power analyses
the sample size of 53, the number of predictors of 6, the
alpha level of p < .05, and Cohen's (1988) criteria of
effect sizes (small [f2 = .02], medium [f2 =
.15], and large [f2 = .35]) were used. The
post-hoc analyses revealed that the statistical power for
the mediation analyses was .09 and .47 to detect small and
medium effects, whereas it was .87 for for detecting large
effects. Hence, there was a high power at the high effect
size level, but a low power at the medium and small effect
size level.
References
Auer M., Gruber G., Mayringer H.,
Wimmer H. (2008). SLS 58: Salzburger Lese-Screening
für die Klassenstufen 58 [SLS 58:
Salzburger Reading-Screening for 5th to 8th graders].
Bern: Huber
Bertrams A., Dickhäuser O.
(2009). Messung dispositioneller
Selbstkontroll-Kapazität: Eine deutsche Adaptation der
Kurzform der Self-Control Scale (SCS-K-D) [Measuring
dispositional self-control capacity: a German adaptation of
the short form of the Self-Control Scale (SCS-K-D)].
Diagnostica 55, 210 10.1026/0012-1924.55.1.2
[Cross Ref]
Blair C. (2002). School readiness:
integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological
conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry.
Am. Psychol. 57, 111127 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Bjorklund D. F., Kipp K. (1996).
Parental investment theory and gender differences in the
evolution of inhibition mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 120,
163188 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.163 [PubMed]
[Cross Ref]
Brdar I., Rijavec M., Loncaric D.
(2006). Goal orientations, coping with school failure and
school achievement. Eur. J. Psychol. Edu. 21, 5370
10.1007/BF03173569 [Cross Ref]
Calkins S. D. (2007). The emergence of
self-regulation: biological and behavioral control
mechanisms supporting toddler competencies, in
Socioemotional Development in the Toddler Years: Transitions
and Transformations, eds Brownell C. A., Kopp C. B.,
editors. (New York, NY: Guilford Press; ),
261284
Cohen J. (1988). Statistical Power
Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum
Cohen M., Ben-Zur H., Rosenfeld M. J.
(2008). Sense of coherence, coping strategies, and test
anxiety as predictors of test performance among college
students. Int. J. Stress Manage. 15, 289303
10.1037/1072-5245.15.3.289 [Cross Ref]
Cole P. M. (1997). The ETS Gender
Study: How Females and Males Perform in Educational
Settings. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing
Service
Cole P. M., Martin S. E., Dennis T. A.
(2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct:
methodological challenges and directions for child
development research. Child Dev. 75, 317333
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x [PubMed] [Cross
Ref]
Cross C. P., Copping L. T., Campbell
A. (2011). Sex differences in impulsivity: a meta-analysis.
Psychol. Bull. 137, 97130 10.1037/a0021591
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Davis T. L. (1995). Gender differences
in masking negative emotions: ability or motivation?
Develop. Psychol. 31, 660667
10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.660 [Cross Ref]
Deary I. J., Strand S., Smith P.,
Fernandes C. (2007). Intelligence and educational
achievement. Intelligence 35, 1321
10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001 [Cross Ref]
Duckworth A. L., Kern M. L. (2011). A
meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control
measures. J. Res. Pers. 45, 259268
10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004 [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Duckworth A., Seligman M. (2005).
Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic
performance of adolescents. Psychol. Sci. 16, 939944
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x [PubMed] [Cross
Ref]
Duckworth A., Seligman M. (2006).
Self-discipline gives girls the edge: gender in
self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. J.
Edu. Psychol. 98, 198208 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198
[Cross Ref]
Eisenberg N., Spinrad T. L. (2004).
Emotion-related regulation: sharpening the definition. Child
Develop. 75, 334339 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00674.x
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Else-Quest N. M., Hyde J. S.,
Goldsmith H. H., Van Hulle C. A. (2006). Gender differences
in temperament: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132,
3372 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33 [PubMed]
[Cross Ref]
Eschenbeck H., Kohlmann C., Lohaus A.
(2007). Gender differences in coping strategies in children
and adolescents. J. Individ. Differ. 28, 1826
10.1027/1614-0001.28.1.18 [Cross Ref]
Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G.,
Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power
analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
sciences. Behav. Res. Meth. 39, 175191
10.3758/BF03193146 [PubMed] [Cross
Ref]
Galdi S., Cadinu M., Tomasetto C.
(2013). The roots of stereotype threat: when automatic
associations disrupt girls' math performance. Child Develop.
Advance online publication. 10.1111/cdev.12128
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Gross J. J., Thompson R. A. (2007).
Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations, in Handbook of
Emotion Regulation, ed Gross J. J., editor. (New York, NY:
Guilford Press; ), 324
Hannover B., Kessels U. (2011). Sind
Jungen die neuen Bildungsverlierer? Empirische Evidenz
für Geschlechterdisparitäten zuungunsten von
Jungen und Erklärungsansätze [Are boys left
behind at school? Reviewing and explaining education-related
gender disparities]. Z. Pädagog. Psychol. 25,
89103 10.1024/1010-0652/a000039 [Cross
Ref]
Hartley B. L., Sutton R. M. (2013). A
stereotype threat account of boys' academic
underachievement. Child Develop. Advance online publication.
10.1111/cdev.12079 [PubMed] [Cross
Ref]
Heikamp T., Trommsdorff G.,
Fäsche A. (2013). Development of self-regulation in
context, in Acting Intentionally and Its Limits:
Individuals, Groups, Institutions, eds Seebaß G.,
Schmitz M., Gollwitzer P. M., editors. (Berlin: De Gruyter;
), 193222 10.1515/9783110284461.193 [Cross
Ref]
Hyde J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and
the psychology of gender differences. Signs J. Women Cult.
Soc. 16, 5573 10.1086/494645 [Cross
Ref]
Karoly P. (1993). Mechanisms of
self-regulation: a systems view. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 44,
2352 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000323 [Cross
Ref]
Keller J., Dauenheimer D. (2003).
Stereotype threat in the classroom: dejection mediates the
disrupting threat effect on women's math performance. Pers.
Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 371381
10.1177/0146167202250218 [PubMed] [Cross
Ref]
Kuhl P., Hannover B. (2012).
Differenzielle Benotungen von Mädchen und Jungen: Der
Einfluss der von der Lehrkraft eingeschätzten Kompetenz
zum selbstgesteuerten Lernen [Different grading for
girls versus boys? Examining the impact of students' ability
for self-regulated learning as perceived by their
teachers]. Z. Entwicklungspsychol. Pädagog.
Psychol. 44, 153162 10.1026/0049-8637/a000066
[Cross Ref]
Lazarus R. S., Folkman S. (1984).
Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY:
Springer
Lohaus A., Eschenbeck H., Kohlmann C.,
Klein-Heßling J. (2006). Fragebogen zur Erhebung von
Stress und Stressbewältigung im Kindes- und Jugendalter
(SSKJ 3-8) [Questionnaire for the Measurement of Stress
and Coping in Children and Adolescents (SSKJ 3-8)].
Göttingen: Hogrefe
Machin S., Pekkarinen T. (2008).
Global sex differences in test score variability. Science
322, 13311332 10.1126/science.1162573 [PubMed]
[Cross Ref]
May P. (2007). HSP 59: Hamburger
Schreib-Probe [HSP 59: Hamburger
Writing-Test]. Seelze: Verlag für pädagogische
Medien
McClelland M. M., Cameron C. E.,
Connor C. M., Farris C. L., Jewkes A. M., Morrison F. J.
(2007). Links between behavioral regulation and
preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills.
Develop. Psychol. 43, 947959
10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947 [PubMed] [Cross
Ref]
McClelland M. M., Ponitz C. C.,
Messersmith E. E., Tominey S. (2010). Self-regulation:
integration of cognition and emotion, in The Handbook of
Life-Span Development: Cognition, Biology, and Methods, Vol.
1, eds Overton W. F., Lerner R. M., editors. (Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley; ), 509553 10.1002/9780470880166.hlsd001015
[Cross Ref]
Mietzel G., Willenberg H. (2000). HST
4/5: Hamburger Schulleistungstest für 4. und 5. Klassen
[HST 4/5: Hamburger school achievement test for 4th and
5th graders]. Göttingen: Hogrefe
Ministerium für Kultus, Jugend
und Sport Baden-Württemberg. (2004). Bildungsstandards
für Deutsch: Gymnasium Klassen 6, 8, 10,
Kursstufe [Educational standards for German: Secondary
school grades 6, 8, 10, course level]. Retrieved
from
http://www.bildung-staerkt-menschen.de/service/downloads/Bildungsstandards/Gym/Gym_D_bs.pdf
Mullola S., Ravaja N., Lipsanen J.,
Hirstiö-Snellman P., Alatupa S.,
Keltikangas-Järvinen L. (2010). Teacher-perceived
temperament and educational competence as predictors of
school grades. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 209214
10.1016/j.lindif.2010.01.008 [Cross Ref]
Pomerantz E. M., Altermatt E. R.,
Saxon J. L. (2002). Making the grade but feeling distressed:
gender differences in academic performance and internal
distress. J. Edu. Psychol. 94, 396404
10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.396 [Cross Ref]
Preacher K. J., Hayes A. F. (2008).
Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models.
Behav. Res. Meth. 40, 879891 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Raffaelli M., Crockett L. J., Shen Y.
(2005). Developmental stability and change in
self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. J. Genet.
Psychol. 166, 5475 10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Schrader F., Helmke A. (2008).
Determinanten der Schulleistung [Determinants of school
performance], in Lehrer-Schüler-Interaktion.
Inhaltsfelder, Forschungsperspektiven und methodische
Zugänge, 2nd Edn ed Schweer M. K. W., editor.
(Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenshaften; ),
285302 10.1007/978-3-531-91104-5_11 [Cross
Ref]
Schunk D. H., Zimmerman B. J. (1997).
Social origins of self-regulatory competence. Edu. Psychol.
32, 195208 10.1207/s15326985ep3204_1 [Cross
Ref]
Silverman I. W. (2003). Gender
differences in delay of gratification: a meta-analysis. Sex
Roles 49, 451463 10.1023/A:1025872421115 [Cross
Ref]
Skinner E. A., Zimmer-Gembeck M. J.
(2007). The development of coping. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58,
119144 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085705
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Spiewak M. (2010). Nichts wie raus!
Jungen sind die Verlierer des Bildungssystems. Das echte
Leben ist die bessere Schule für sie [Let's get out
of here! Boys are the losers of the educational system. Real
life is the better school for them]. DIE ZEIT,
2729
Spinath B., Freudenthaler H. H.,
Neubauer A. C. (2010). Domain-specific school achievement in
boys and girls as predicted by intelligence, personality and
motivation. Pers. Individ. Differ. 48, 481486
10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.028 [Cross Ref]
Stanat P., Kunter M. (2003).
Kompetenzerwerb, Bildungsbeteiligung und Schullaufbahn von
Mädchen und Jungen im Ländervergleich
[Acquisition of competencies, participation in education
and school career of girls and boys in comparison of German
Federal States], in PISA 2000: Ein differenzierter Blick
auf die Länder der Bundesrepublik Deutschland, eds
Baumert J., Artelt C., Klieme E., Neubrand M., Prenzel M.,
Schiefele U., et al., editors. (Opladen: Leske + Budrich; ),
211242
Stanat P., Pant H. A., Böhme K.,
Richter D., editors. (eds.). (2012). Kompetenzen von
Schülerinnen und Schülern am Ende der vierten
Jahrgangsstufe in den Fächern Deutsch und Mathematik:
Ergebnisse des IQB-Ländervergleichs 2011
[Competencies of girls and boys at the end of 4th school
grade in the subjects German and mathematics: Results of the
IQB-comparison of German Federal States 2011].
Münster: Waxmann
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2011).
Fachserie 11: Bildung und Kultur, Reihe 1: Allgemeinbildende
Schulen [Technical series 11: Education and culture,
Volume 1: General schools]. Wiesbaden, Germany:
Statistisches Bundesamt. Retrieved from
https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/Thematisch/BildungForschungKultur/Schulen/AllgemeinbildendeSchulen.html
Suchodoletz A. V., Trommsdorff G.,
Heikamp T., Wieber F., Gollwitzer P. M. (2009). Transition
to school: the role of kindergarten children's behavior
regulation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 19, 561566
10.1016/j.lindif.2009.07.006 [Cross Ref]
Tangney J. P., Baumeister R. F., Boone
A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment,
less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J.
Pers. 72, 271322 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
[PubMed] [Cross Ref]
Tsukayama E., Duckworth A., Kim B.
(2012). Resisting everything except temptation: evidence and
an explanation for domain-specific impulsivity. Eur. J.
Pers. 26, 318334 10.1002/per.841 [Cross
Ref]
Trommsdorff G. (2009). Culture and
development of self-regulation. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass
3, 687701 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00209.x [Cross
Ref]
Trommsdorff G. (in press).
Socialization of self-regulation for achievement in cultural
context: a developmental perspective on the Asian miracle,
in Asia's Educational Miracle: Psychological, Social, and
Cultural Perspectives, eds Kim U., Park Y.-S., editors. (New
York, NY: Springer; ).
Weiß R. H. (2006). CFT 20-R.
Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 (CFT 20-R) [Basic
Intelligence Scale 2 (CFT 20-R)]. Göttingen:
Hogrefe
Zeidner M. (1995). Coping with
examination stress: Resources, strategies, outcomes. Anxiety
Stress Coping Int. J. 8, 279298
10.1080/10615809508249379 [Cross Ref]
Zimmerman B. J. (1990). Self-regulated
learning and academic achievement: an overview. Edu.
Psychol. 25, 317 10.1207/s15326985ep2501_2 [Cross
Ref]
Zimmerman B. J., Schunk D. H. (2011).
Self-regulated learning and performance: an introduction and
an overview, in Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and
Performance, eds Zimmerman B. J., Schunk D. H., editors.
(New York, NY: Routledge; ), 112
Source: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3713251/
Boys
Lack of Effort in School Tied to College Gender Gap
When it comes to college education, men are falling
behind by standing still.
The proportion of men receiving
college degrees has stagnated, while women have thrived
under the new economic and social realities in the United
States and elsewhere, according to two sociologists who have
written a new book on the subject.
The world has changed around
boys, and they have not adapted as well as girls, said
Claudia Buchmann, a professor of sociology at Ohio State
University and co-author of The Rise of Women: The Growing
Gender Gap in Education and What it Means for American
Schools (Russell Sage Foundation, 2013).
Buchmann and co-author Thomas DiPrete,
professor of sociology at Columbia University, spent more
than a decade researching the education gender gap. They
wanted to find out why women are now getting more college
degrees than ever before, while the proportion of young men
doing so hasnt changed much in more than 50
years.
In 1960, 65 percent of all
bachelors degrees were awarded to men. By 2010, the
gender positions reversed and women received 57 percent of
all bachelors degrees.
Some commentators blame schools and
argue that schools have become too feminized and
dont support the way that boys learn. Some have
asserted that single-sex education is the best way to help
improve boys academic achievement.
But theres little evidence to
support these arguments, Buchmann said.
Schools havent changed
that much. Boys have long underachieved in school compared
to girls, but it mattered less when they could get good
blue-collar jobs without a college degree, Buchmann
said.
In the last few decades, as
those good blue-collar jobs have declined, that boys
performance in school has become a bigger
issue.
Meanwhile, new job opportunities have
opened for women in our society, giving girls the incentive
to use their better academic skills to earn college
degrees.
Boys underachievement compared
to girls has nothing to do with intelligence. Study after
study shows that boys and girls are very similar in terms of
cognitive ability.
But what is striking is that at
every level of cognitive ability, boys are getting lower
grades than girls. It is not about ability it is
about effort and engagement, Buchmann said.
More girls than boys report that they
like school and that good grades are important to them. They
also study more than boys.
Success in academics, like
success in sports, requires time and effort. Because boys
put forth less effort and are less engaged, they get lower
grades and are less likely to get through college,
Buchmann said.
Some of boys underperformance is
related to outdated views of masculinity that devalue hard
work and effort in school, she said. This is particularly
true for boys from blue-collar and lower-class families.
Working class fathers may reinforce the idea that school is
feminizing because, for them, masculinity is more about
physical strength and manual labor than about getting good
grades.
Many boys from middle-class families,
whose fathers have managerial and white-collar jobs, often
develop an instrumental approach to school,
Buchmann said. Regardless of how much they like school, they
have learned how to do well in school in order to get a
well-paying job and achieve material success.
For these boys, notions of what
it means to be a man are much more in tune with what is
required to be successful in todays economy, she
said.
Buchmann said the best solution to the
education gender gap is to focus efforts on the middle
third of students many of whom are boys - who
have the ability to go to college, but who are not honing
the academic skills they will need to successfully graduate.
These are generally students who are getting mostly
B grades in their classes, with a few
Cs.
The top third of students are those,
mostly from white-collar families, who are already on track
to successfully finish college, while the bottom third
dont have the resources and skills to realistically
finish a four-year degree.
In order to reach these boys in the
middle third, the answer isnt single-sex classrooms,
or making schools more boy-friendly, Buchmann
said.
This taps into those narrow
notions of what boys and men are like. That is going to
backfire, she said.
Boys have long
underachieved in school compared to girls, but it
mattered less when they could get good blue-collar jobs
without a college degree.
Instead, we need schools to
expect high levels of effort and academic achievement of all
students, including boys. Schools need to break down the
gendered stereotypes that say that real men dont work
hard in school.
Schools also need to do a better job
of teaching students about the pathways through college to a
good job. They need to make clear what kinds of grades
students will need, and what kind of classes they need to
take, to get the job they want to have.
This should start in elementary
school, but should be especially emphasized in middle and
high schools.
Many boys say they expect to go
to college, and many will enroll, but their expectations
about what it will take to succeed are way off. They
underestimate the work and effort they need to put
forth, she said.
In one survey, 65 percent of boys in
8th grade expected they would get at least a bachelors
degree.
Not even half of the boys who
think they are going to get a college degree will actually
do so, Buchmann said.
Those years from 7th to 12th
grade are crucial for really learning good study skills,
learning how to apply yourself to your studies, and how to
stay motivated even when the schoolwork is not particularly
fun.
The good news is that the same changes
that will help more boys achieve college success will help
girls as well.
This is not a zero-sum
game, Buchmann said. Helping boys to succeed in
school wont hurt girls. It is all about closing the
gender gap.
Source: researchnews.osu.edu/archive/riseofwomen2.htm
Sugar and spice
and
math under-achievement? Why classrooms, not
girls, need fixing
Mathematics has a girl problem. Although girls achieve
at equal levels to boys in middle and high school, many
girls stop taking math as soon as they can. Girls are also
much less likely than boys to enter math-intensive college
majors and, later, careers. Gender researchers have shown
that the root of this girl problem is not differences in
innate math skills, but rather the contexts in which
students learn mathcontexts that give girls less
encouragement and less confidence in their math abilities.
Eager to address this girl problem, educators and
policymakers usually respond: okay, so how do we fix the
girls? But, according to Jo Boaler, its the math
classrooms, not the girls, which really need
fixing.
Boaler, a Professor of Math Education
in Stanfords School of Education, explained in a
recent presentation why traditional ways of teaching math
through rote memorization just arent cutting it. Her
research shows that by simply changing the way math is
taught, gender differences in math achievement and math
confidence disappear.
Are girls really worse at
math?
Boaler is often asked whether the
girl problem is just a gene problem.
Americans tend to understand gender differences in math
achievement as unchangingunchangeabledifferences
in the way that boys and girls think. Girls just
arent hard wired for math, some say. But
decades of research proves this assumption wrong. For one,
gender gaps in math achievement have rapidly declined over
the last centuryfar outpacing any possible shifts in
human genetics. Additionally, gender differences are
country-specific: in some European nations, boys and
girls math performance is equal. In places like
Iceland, girls outperform boys. If gender differences vary
by culture, then can these differences really be genetic?
Perhaps most compelling, researchers examined over 250
separate studies of gender differences in math and found no
appreciable differences in ability once the number of math
courses boys and girls took was held constant.
Many educational decision-makers now
understand that girls preferences are not a result of
genetics but rather the different ways boys and girls are
treated by peers, teachers and parents vis-à-vis
math. To address this issue, schools abound with math
camps, extracurricular activities, and special (often pink)
toys meant to develop girls confidence and interest in
math. But, Boaler asks, if the learning contexts are the
problem, why are most policies aimed at addressing gender
differences in math still trying to fix girls?
Fix the classrooms, not the
girls
Educational environments in which
girls and boys learn math need changing, says Boaler. The
majority of math classrooms in the U.S. take a traditional
approach to learning, where teachers introduce students to
progressively more difficult mathematical procedures.
Students are expected to memorize these procedures and then
execute them on homework and tests. Math problems are
usually the closed-ended type where a single answer can be
circled at the end, and math procedures are usually taught
by extracting them from real-world situations where a person
might actually need to use those procedures. For most of
us, save the obtuse word problem here and there, learning
math meant scribbling down, memorizing, and recapitulating
the long strings of equations our teachers wrote on the
board.
Just because this is the way most of
us were taught math does not mean its the only way,
the best way, or the most gender equitable way. Boaler
asks: what if we identified the learning environments that
produced the most equitable and successful results and then
used those learning environments as templates for the way
math should be taught?
Boalers research actually
identified such a learning environment. She studied
approaches to math education at two otherwise
nearly-identical high schools in England: Amber
Hill and Phoenix Park. Amber Hill
approached math the traditional waystudents copied
down formulas from the board, completed worksheets, and were
split up into one of eight ability groups. At this school,
boys did better in math than girls.
Things were different at Phoenix Park.
Instead of a traditional environment, students learned math
through collaboration, working together with their
classmates to solve complex, multi-dimensional, open-ended
problems. At Phoenix Park, boys and girls performed equally
well in math and both boys and girls scored at higher levels
than the students who had learned math traditionally.
But what about the
boys?
Skeptics might argue that this erasure
of gender differences was achieved because boys math
performance slipped in the Phoenix Park context. But,
thats simply not the caseBoaler found that,
although the improvement was smaller in magnitude, boys at
Phoenix Park also scored slightly better than boys at Amber
Hill. If a learning environment produces a more equitable
learning experience for one group of students without
negatively affecting the other groups math
achievement, why wouldnt we adopt this new
approach?
Boaler explains that there is a
surprisingly high level of resistance among parents,
teachers, and principals to this new way of teaching math.
Part of this resistance may be due to the belief that math
is a rite of passage of sorts, which builds character and
perseverance in young people. I struggled through my
math courses, some say, and so should
todays students. But the fact is, Boaler
explains, compared to other academic
subjectsEnglish, science, etcthe way we teach
math to children is very different from the way math
education researchers have identified as the most effective
way to teach math. By realigning math education to be
more like the gender-equitable learning environments at
Phoenix Park, we can move the dialogand the
blamefrom whats wrong with girls to how we can
make math education better for everyone.
Of course, not all parents have the
ability to place their children in gender-equitable math
learning environments. For those parents, Boaler has an
important piece of advice: parents should emphasize to
their children that being good at math is an achievement,
not a gift. Once studentsespecially
girlsunderstand that being good at math is something
that one can earn, they are likely to be more confident in
their math abilities, and less willing to give up on math.
oys in math in high school. Three
students were accepted from our high school to MIT and they
are all girls. One person was accepted to Stanford and she
is a girl. If you look at the girls in middle school now,
they are passing the boys. Do you have any data on how boys
are being left behind now in middle school and high school?
I never see scholarships specifically for boys;
Source: gender.stanford.edu/news/2012/sugar-and-spice-and%E2%80%A6-math-under-achievement
Gender and
Academic Achievement
Most studies show that, on average, girls do better in
school than boys. Girls get higher grades and complete high
school at a higher rate compared to boys (Jacobs, 2002).
Standardized achievement tests also show that females are
better at spelling and perform better on tests of literacy,
writing, and general knowledge (National Center for
Education Statistics, 2003). An international aptitude test
administered to fourth graders in 35 countries, for example,
showed that females outscored males on reading literacy in
every country. Although there were no differences between
boys and girls in fourth grade on mathematics, boys began to
perform better than girls on science tests in fourth grade
(International Association for the Evaluation of Education
Achievement, n.d.). Girls continue to exhibit higher verbal
ability throughout high school, but they begin to lose
ground to boys after fourth grade on tests of both
mathematical and science ability. These gender differences
in math and science achievement have implications for
girls future careers and have been a source of concern
for educators everywhere.
Download
Article
During the past decade, there has been
a concerted effort to find out why there is a shortage of
women in the science, math, engineering, and technical
fields (AAUW, 1992). In 1995, 22% of Americas
scientists and engineers were women, compared to half of the
social scientists. Women who do pursue careers in science,
engineering, and mathematics most often choose fields in the
biological sciences, where they represent 40% of the
workforce, with smaller percentages found in mathematics or
computer science (33%), the physical sciences (22%), and
engineering (9%) (National Science Board, 1998).
Part of the explanation can be traced
to gender differences in the cognitive abilities of
middle-school students. In late elementary school, females
outperform males on several verbal skills tasks: verbal
reasoning, verbal fluency, comprehension, and understanding
logical relations (Hedges & Nowell, 1995). Males, on the
other hand, outperform females on spatial skills tasks such
as mental rotation, spatial perception, and spatial
visualization (Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Males also
perform better on mathematical achievement tests than
females. However, gender differences do not apply to all
aspects of mathematical skill. Males and females do equally
well in basic math knowledge, and girls actually have better
computational skills. Performance in mathematical reasoning
and geometry shows the greatest difference (Fennema, Sowder,
& Carpenter, 1999). Males also display greater
confidence in their math skills, which is a strong predictor
of math performance (Casey, Nuttall, & Pezaris,
2001).
The poorer mathematical reasoning
skills exhibited by many female adolescents have several
educational implications. Beginning at age 12, girls begin
to like math and science less and to like language arts and
social studies more than do boys (Kahle & Lakes, 2003;
Sadker & Sadker, 1994). They also do not expect to do as
well in these subjects and attribute their failures to lack
of ability (Eccles, Barber, Jozefowicz, Malenchuk, &
Vida, 1999). By high school, girls self-select out of
higher-level, academic-track math and science
courses, such as calculus and chemistry. One of the
long-term consequences of these choices is that girls lack
the prerequisite high school math and science courses
necessary to pursue certain majors in college (e.g.,
engineering, computer science). Consequently, the number of
women who pursue advanced degrees in these fields is
significantly reduced (Halpern, 2004).
Some researchers, on the one hand,
argue that the gender gap in mathematics is biologically
driven. Selected research shows that prenatal hormones
circulating in the brain encourage differential development
in the hemispheres of male and female fetuses (Berenbaum,
Korman, & Leveroni, 1995). Others believe intelligence
has its roots in genetics (Plomin, 2000). There is evidence,
however, that sociocultural factors may influence
girls attitudes toward math and science. For example,
parents tend to view math as more important for sons and
language arts and social studies as more important for
daughters (Andre, Whigham, Hendrickson, & Chambers,
1999). Parents are more likely to encourage their sons to
take advanced high school courses in chemistry, mathematics,
and physics and have higher expectations for their success
(Wigfield, Battle, Keller, & Eccles, 2002).
Teacher characteristics and the
classroom environment also have been identified as
contributors to this gender gap. Seventh and eighth graders
attending math and science camps identified a math or
science teacher as a person who has made math,
science, or engineering interesting for them (Gilbert,
1996, p. 491). Unfortunately, many females report being
passed over in classroom discussions, not encouraged by the
teacher, and made to feel stupid (Sadker & Sadker,
1994). Classroom environments can be made to feel more
girl-friendly by incorporating
Fortunately, sex differences in
mathematical reasoning have begun to decline, and
females enrollments are up in math and science courses
(Campbell, Hombo, & Mazzeo, 2000; Freeman, 2004).
Programs designed to interest girls in math and science and
that demonstrate how this knowledge will allow them to help
others appear to be working.
Excerpt from Middle Childhood
Development: A Contextual Approach, by M.J. Zembar, L.B.
Blume, 2009 edition, p. 212-215.
Source: www.education.com/reference/article/gender-academic-achievement/

Gender
Segregation: Separate But Effective?
Last October, more than 450 public school teachers,
principals and central administrators from across the United
States as well as from Argentina, Bermuda, Canada and
Poland came together in Atlanta, Georgia, for the
fifth annual convention of the National Association for
Single Sex Public Education.
Dozens of presentations extolled the
superiority of gender-segregated classrooms and entire
schools, with lecture titles such as, Burps, Farts and
Snot: Teaching Chemistry To Middle School Boys, and
Just Dont Say SEX tips on how
to implement single-gender programs in conservative, rural
communities.
Attendees ranged from Chicago and
Philadelphia inner-city high school teachers to elementary
school principals from small towns in Idaho and Indiana.
They represented a fraction of recent converts to the Single
Sex Public Education (SSPE) movement, which has expanded at
a remarkable pace.
In 2002, only 11 public schools in the
United States had gender-segregated classrooms. As of
December 2009, there were more than 550.
The movement is based on the
hypothesis that hard-wired differences in the ways that male
and female brains develop and function in childhood through
adolescence require classrooms in which boys and girls are
not only separated by gender, but also taught according to
radically different methods.
For example, SSPE doctrine calls for
teachers in male classrooms to be constantly moving and
speaking in a loud voice, even to the point of shouting,
while teachers in female classes should be still and use a
calming tone. This differentiation stems from the central
tenet of SSPE ideology that young males thrive on
competition and confrontation, while young females require a
more nurturing and cooperative learning
environment.
When most young boys are exposed
to threat and confrontation, their senses sharpen, and they
feel a thrill, explains Dr. Leonard Sax, the founder
and executive director of the National Associate for Single
Sex Public Education. When most young girls are
exposed to such stimuli, however, they feel dizzy and
yucky.
In a landmark essay published in the
Spring 2006 edition of Educational Horizons, just as the
SSPE movement was gaining strong momentum, Dr. Sax detailed
the different ways elementary school teachers should address
their students in gender-segregated classes. [The
teacher] may move right in front of a boy and say,
Whats your answer, Mr. Jackson? Give it to
me! Far from being intimidated, boys are energized by
this teaching style. With girls [teachers should]
speak more softly, use first names, terms of endearment and
fewer direct commands: Lisa, sweetie, its time
to open your book. Emily, darling, would you please sit down
for me and join us in this exercise?
The title of Dr. Saxs essay was
Six Degrees of Separation, a reference to the
SSPE guideline that while the perfect ambient temperature
for a male classroom is 69 degrees Fahrenheit, females learn
most effectively at 75 degrees.
Heroic Behavior vs. Wedding
Cakes
Separating boys and girls is a
longstanding tradition at private and parochial schools. The
concept began to gain traction in American public schools
earlier this decade as schools began to experiment with SSPE
in oft-desperate attempts to reduce disciplinary problems
and improve test scores. The Department of Education
accelerated the trend in 2006 by altering the Title IX
provision of the No Child Left Behind Act to ease
restrictions on gender-segregated education in public
schools.
Since then, advocates like Dr. Sax, a
child psychologist who never set foot in a classroom as a
teacher, have stepped up their promotion of SSPE as a
panacea for public education. With scant evidence backing
them up, they herald SSPE as the most effective way to
narrow the achievement gaps between rich and poor students
and black and white students that persist eight years after
the passage of No Child Left Behind.
Although SSPE programs are now in
place at schools in 39 states and the District of Columbia,
they are particularly popular in urban districts with large
minority populations, and most concentrated in the
Southeastern U.S. South Carolina has 173 SSPE schools, by
far the most of any state.
Last year, the largest school system
in Alabama, the Mobile County Public School System, with
66,000 students, implemented SSPE programs in eight of its
93 schools with no parental notification. The most extreme
program was at Hankins Middle School in Theodore, Alabama,
where boys and girls ate lunch at different times and were
prohibited from speaking to one another on school
grounds.
Hankins teachers were directed to
create competitive, high-energy classrooms for
boys and cooperative, quiet classrooms for
girls. Boys were to be taught heroic behavior.
Girls were to learn good character. Sixth-grade
language arts exercises called for boys to brainstorm action
words used in sports. Girls were instructed to describe
their dream wedding cake. Electives were gender-specific.
Boys took computer applications. Girls took drama. No
exceptions.
Mark Jones, whose son Jacob attends
Hankins, said that when he complained to the principal about
the changes, she told him they were necessary because
boys and girls brains were so different
they needed different curriculum.
Segregating boys and girls
didnt make things any better for our children. In fact
they made things worse, Jones said. Our kids
were basically being taught ideas about gender that come
from the Dark Ages.
Another parent, Terry Stevens, also
objected. The real world is integrated, and its
important to both me and my son that he learn in a coed
environment, Stevens said.
Other parents and students disagreed.
You learn more like this, 11-year-old Brenda
Orduna told the Mobile Press-Register after making the honor
roll at the end of the first quarter for the first time in
her academic career. When boys are around, youre
shy. And you wont ask questions if you dont get
it.
Muddled Results
The Mobile County SSPE experiment was
short-lived. The district terminated all eight of its SSPE
programs last March after the American Civil Liberties Union
threatened to file a lawsuit on behalf of Jones and Stevens.
The ACLU took the position that the Hankins program violated
even the slackened Title IX provision. (The other seven
Mobile County SSPE programs either offered all elective
courses to both genders, in single-sex classrooms, or made
their SSPE programs optional, with co-ed alternatives. At
Hankins, they were mandatory.)
While schools might think that
sex-segregated classes will be a quick fix for failing
schools, in reality they are inherently unequal and
shortchange both boys and girls, said Emily Martin,
Deputy Director of the ACLU Womens Rights Program.
There is no reliable evidence that segregating
students by sex improves learning by either
sex.
It is fair to say the supposed
benefits of gender-segregated education in public schools
claimed by SSPE supporters are unproven. On the other hand,
there is no solid evidence that SSPE is harmful to the
learning process of either gender, as critics argue. SSPE is
such a relatively new phenomenon that no major credible
studies have been conducted of its long-term efficacy.
Likewise, research into gender-segregated education in
general, let alone the controversial teaching methods
promoted by the SSPE movement, has been
inconclusive.
A 2006 study completed at the College
of Education at Arizona State University showed that most of
the research into gender-segregated education thus far has
been of questionable value. According to the ASU study, the
research
is mostly flawed by failure to control
for important variables such as class, financial status,
selective admissions, religious values, prior learning or
ethnicity. The ASU study also found that the
methodology of less than 2 percent of the more than 2,000
quantitative studies of gender-segregated education was of
high enough quality to meet the standards of the National
Center for Education Statistics.
In 2005 the Department of Education
released a comprehensive meta-analysis of gender-segregated
education scholarship, titled Single Sex Versus
Coeducational Schooling: A Systematic Review. The DOE
found the results equivocal.
There is some support for the
premise that single-sex schooling can be helpful especially
for certain outcomes related to academic achievement and
more positive academic aspirations, the DOE reported.
For many outcomes, there is no evidence of either
benefit or harm. There is limited support for the view that
single-sex schooling may be harmful.
The DOE report included the caveat
that most research into gender-segregated education has been
conducted in private Catholic schools, which hardly makes
for an apples-to-apples comparison to public
education.
Sex segregation doesnt
make public schools more like private schools, says
Allison Neal, staff attorney with the ACLU of Alabama.
If some private schools provide a better education,
its because of their resources, not because
theyre single sex.
A Self-Confidence
Thing
Dr. Sax counters the mixed results of
the Department of Education analysis by pointing out that
most of the studies reviewed by the DOE involved merely
segregating boys and girls in different classrooms without
deploying SSPE teaching methods.
The most obvious explanation for
the variation is that merely placing girls and boys in
separate classrooms accomplishes little, he said.
For the single-sex format to lead to improvements in
academic performance, teachers must understand the
hard-wired differences in how girls and boys learn and
incorporate the best practices for all-female classrooms and
all-male classrooms.
Dr. Sax has made a cottage industry of
training public school teachers in those classroom
practices. He maintains that two days of training, 14 hours
total, is all thats needed to prepare the staff of a
public school to switch from coeducation to SSPE. Since
2002, Dr. Sax by his own count has led such two-day
conversion seminars for more than 300 schools in the United
States, Canada and Mexico.
One of them was Carman Trails, an
elementary school in the Parkway School District, which is
in the St. Louis area. Despite a lack of test data to prove
the program is working, SSPE at Carman Trails has won over
teachers, parents and students. The program is expanding.
When it began two years ago, it was limited to first grade.
For the 2008-2009 academic year, first- and second-graders
were segregated by gender. In February 2009, at the urging
of enthusiastic parents, principal Chris Raeker grew the
program to include the third grade.
Raeker said that since implementing
the SSPE program, fewer boys are being sent to the
principals office, their overall attendance is up and
they are participating in school clubs in higher numbers.
First-grade teacher Alicia Wall said the program is
benefiting girls in different ways. I definitely see a
self-confidence thing, Wall said. The girls are
ready to learn and ready to work. In coed classes,
theyre afraid to say something. Theyre afraid to
be wrong.
The anecdotal success stories from
schools like Carman Trails fail to sway opponents of SSPE,
which include members of the American Civil Liberties Union
and the American Association of University Women. They argue
that SSPE is not a silver bullet for improving performance
in public schools. Further, they point out that segregating
students by race based on supposed differences in brain
function between, for example, Asian students and African
American students, would be decried as racist and arouse
widespread protests.
School districts across the
country are experimenting with sex-segregated programs,
which rely on questionable brain science theories based on
outdated gender stereotypes, said the ACLUs
Martin. Instead, these districts should focus on
efforts that we know can improve all students
education, like smaller classes and more teacher training
and parental involvement.
Source: www.tolerance.org/magazine/number-37-spring-2010/feature/gender-segregation-separate-effective

Resources
Gender
differences in adolescent academic achievement, interests,
values and life-role expectations
JL Lupart*, E Cannon, JA Telfer - High Ability Studies, 2004
- Taylor & Francis
To ensure greatest confidence in our
data we were careful to compare our results with those ...
girls appear to have a better attitude toward learning for
learning's sake as compared to boys ... Next, a comparison
across the three subject areas will be presented and gender
and grade ...
Gender
differences in mathematics performance: a
meta-analysis.
JS Hyde, E Fennema, SJ Lamon - Psychological bulletin, 1990
- psycnet.apa.org
The psychology of gender: Advances
through meta-analysis. ... A comparison of the relationships
between the level of acceptance of sex-role stereotyping and
achievement and attitudes ... Differential prediction of
academic achievement in elementary and junior high school by
sex. ...
Age
and gender differences in children's self-and task
perceptions during elementary school
J Eccles, A Wigfield, RD Harold
- Child
, 1993 -
Wiley Online Library
(not at all good, very good) MU.ABIL.2
Compared to most of ... It also allows the researcher to
compare the fit of the same model across various groups. ...
These comparative tests can be ordered from least to most
restrictive, with the least restrictive test comparing
whether the factor ...
Early
gender differences in self-regulation and academic
achievement.
JS Matthews, CC Ponitz, FJ Morrison - Journal of Educational
, 2009 - psycnet.apa.org
First, how does a direct measure of
behavioral self-regulation compare with teacher ratings of
... self-regulation at kindergarten entry were expected to
demonstrate greater gains compared with boys ... achievement
were not expected to be as large as those for gender
differences in ...
Gender
differences in gifted and average-ability students comparing
girls' and boys' achievement, self-concept, interest, and
motivation in mathematics
F Preckel, T Goetz, R Pekrun, M Kleine - Gifted Child
Quarterly, 2008 - gcq.sagepub.com
Comparing Girls' and Boys'
Achievement, Self-Concept, ... This study investigated
gender differences in gifted sixth graders, as compared with
gender differ- ences in a ... Table 2 Means, Standard
Deviations, and Effect Sizes for the Comparison of
Achievement (Grades and Test ...
Longitudinal
structural equation models of academic self-concept and
achievement: Gender differences in the development of math
and English constructs
HW Marsh, AS Yeung - American Educational Research Journal,
1998 - aer.sagepub.com
subject, and an internal
(ipsative-like) reference, in which students compare their
performances ... More recently, Feingold (1994) compared
results from three meta-analyses of gender ... standardized
achievement tests, there have been surprisingly few studies
comparing these two ...
The
impact of job applicants' facial maturity, gender, and
academic achievement on hiring
recommendations
LA Zebrowitz, DR Tenenbaum
- Journal of Applied
, 1991 - Wiley Online Library
for male than female applicants,
although all were significant for both genders, all ps ...
an advantage for both jobs, the effects of facial maturity
and gender depended upon ... Compared with applicants who
were male or maturefaced, female or babyfaced applicants
were rated as ...
Cultural
capital, gender, and school success: The role of
habitus SA
Dumais - Sociology of education, 2002 - JSTOR
upon cultural capital and on the
inclination to invest in the academic market" (Bourdieu 1973
... Moreover, one's habitus is also gendered as a result of
the possibilities available to each ... of habitus may be
stronger for male students because (according to gender-
socialization theory ...
The nature, effects,
and relief of mathematics anxiety
R Hembree - Journal for research in mathematics education,
1990 - JSTOR Each comparison was drawn in terms of an effect
size; then related groups were analyzed through ... How do
its levels compare between courses and respecting college
majors? ... Thirteen studies of college mathe- matics were
found, comparing the test scores of students with high
...
A
multifaceted academic self-concept: Its hierarchical
structure and its relation to academic
achievement.
HW Marsh, BM Byrne, RJ Shavelson - Journal of educational
, 1988 - psycnet.apa.org
The model further posits an internal
comparison process for which students compare their self ...
Because math achievement and verbal achievement are compared
with each other, it is the ... and when math achievement is
better than verbal achievement (the internal comparison).
...
Gender
differences in verbal ability: A
meta-analysis.
JS Hyde, MC Linn - Psychological bulletin, 1988 -
psycnet.apa.org
A comparison of composition scores of
third-grade children with reading skills, pre-kindergarten
verbal ability, self-concept, and sex. ... In C. Ounsted
& DC Taylor (Eds.), Gender differences: Their ontogeny
and significance(pp. 123-157). ... New York: Academic Press.
...
The
path through math: Course sequences and academic performance
at the intersection of race-ethnicity and
gender C
Riegle-Crumb - American journal of education (Chicago,
Ill.), 2006 - ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
Comparing this to model 2 for male
students, the main effects for failure and high grades are
comparable for female students and male ... While African
American and Latino students of both genders generally start
high school in lower math courses compared with their white
...
The
family-school relation and the child's school
performance
DL Stevenson, DP Baker - Child development, 1987 -
JSTOR
Compared to other so- cial
characteristics of families, such as family size, income,
and ... tended to choose college preparatory courses for
their child independent of the child's academic performance.
... interrela- tions among parental involvement, age of the
child, and gender of the ...
The
academic achievement of adolescents from immigrant families:
The role of family background, attitudes, and
behavior
AJ Fuligni - Child development, 1997 - Wiley Online
Library
students try to over- look the
difficulties of their current experiences by comparing them
to ... and ethnicity were dummy coded, with the categories
listed in the table compared to the ... To compare the
school performance of the two generations of students from
immigrant families (?rst ... researchgate.net
[PDF]
Gender
differences in Internet use patterns and Internet
application preferences: A two-sample
comparison EB
Weiser - CyberPsychology and Behavior, 2000 -
online.liebertpub.com
.testing of college students provides
a known comparison base against which to compare the general
pop ... represented a relatively young and inexperienced
sample of Internet users in comparison to the ... see Table
2). Duncan multi- ple-range tests (p 5 .05) comparing age
group ...
Expectancyvalue
theory of achievement motivation
A
Wigfield, JS Eccles - Contemporary educational psychology,
2000 - Elsevier
These definitions are compared to
those of related constructs, including self-efficacy,
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and interest. ...
Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. ... Gender
differences in sport involvement: Applying the Eccles'
expectancy-value model. ...
Growing
minds: The effect of a school gardening program on the
science achievement of elementary
students
CD Klemmer, TM Waliczek
-
, 2005 -
horttech.ashspublications.orgwere then analyzed using
analysis of vari- ance (ANOVA) methods to compare scores.
... Table 4. An analysis of variance comparing mean student
science achievement scores z within ... scores for females
in the experimental group were 4.31 points higher compared
to females in ...
Gender
differences in personality: a
meta-analysis. A
Feingold - Psychological bulletin, 1994 -
psycnet.apa.org
69-119). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press. ... Cross-cultural comparison of sex-related
differences on intelligence tests: A meta-analysis. Journal
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 18(3), 283-314. ... Gender
differences in eating attitudes, body concept, and
self-esteem among models. ...
Gender
differences in self-efficacy and attitudes toward
computers
T Busch - Journal of educational computing research, 1995 -
jec.sagepub.com
Gardener, Discenza, and Dukes compared
CAS with three other computer attitude scales and
recommended CAS for research if it contains sub- scales
measuring the constructs of interest [45]. ... 39.
V. Makrakis. Cross-cultural Comparison of Gender Differences
in Attitude ...
Relation
of self-efficacy expectations to academic achievement and
persistence.
RW Lent, SD Brown, KC Larkin - Journal of counseling
psychology, 1984 - psycnet.apa.org
In every comparison, differences
favored high-self-efficacy subjects. ... of the high-level
and each of the high-strength subjects persisted for all
four quarters, compared to 58 ... Finally, to explore gender
differences in self-efficacy, data on the four scales were
subjected to a repeated ...
Source: scholar.google.com/scholar?q=comparing+genders+in+academic+achievement&hl=en&as_sdt=0&as_vis=1&oi=scholart&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwjzg72igrLNAhVISlIKHTl0CasQgQMIGjAA

©2007-2023,
www.TheCitizensWhoCare.org/academic-achievement.html
|
|